12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

276 EXPERIMENTS AND META-ANALYSIS<br />

and, hence, inferring causality is more contestable,<br />

but they have the attraction of taking place in a<br />

natural setting. Extraneous variables may include,<br />

for example:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

participant factors: they may differ on important<br />

characteristics between the control and<br />

experimental groups<br />

intervention factors: the intervention may not<br />

be exactly the same for all participants, varying,<br />

for example, in sequence, duration, degree<br />

of intervention and assistance, and other<br />

practices and contents<br />

situational factors: the experimental conditions<br />

may differ.<br />

These can lead to experimental error, in which<br />

the results may not be due to the independent<br />

variables in question.<br />

The pretest-post-test control and<br />

experimental group design<br />

A complete exposition of experimental designs<br />

is beyond the scope of this chapter. In<br />

the brief outline that follows, we have selected<br />

one design from the comprehensive treatment<br />

of the subject by Campbell and Stanley<br />

(1963) in order to identify the essential features<br />

of what they term a ‘true experimental’<br />

and what Kerlinger (1970) refers to as a ‘good’<br />

design. Along with its variants, the chosen design<br />

is commonly used in educational experimentation<br />

(see http://www.routledge.com/textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/<br />

9780415368780 – Chapter 13, file 13.3. ppt).<br />

The pretest-post-test control group design can<br />

be represented as:<br />

Experimental RO 1 X O 2<br />

Control RO 3 O 4<br />

Kerlinger (1970) observes that, in theory, random<br />

assignment to E and C conditions controls all<br />

possible independent variables. In practice, of<br />

course, it is only when enough subjects are<br />

included in the experiment that the principle<br />

of randomization has a chance to operate as<br />

a powerful control. However, the effects of<br />

Box 13.2<br />

The effects of randomization<br />

Select twenty cards from a pack, ten red and ten black.<br />

Shuffle and deal into two ten-card piles. Now count the<br />

number of red cards and black cards in either pile and<br />

record the results. Repeat the whole sequence many<br />

times, recording the results each time.<br />

You will soon convince yourself that the most likely<br />

distribution of reds and blacks in a pile is five in each:<br />

the next most likely, six red (or black) and four black<br />

(or red); and so on. You will be lucky (or unlucky for the<br />

purposes of the demonstration!) to achieve one pile of<br />

red and the other entirely of black cards. The probability<br />

of this happening is 1 in 92,378. On the other hand, the<br />

probability of obtaining a ‘mix’ of not more than six of<br />

one colour and four of the other is about 82 in 100.<br />

If you now imagine the red cards to stand for the<br />

‘better’ ten children and the black cards for the ‘poorer’<br />

ten children in a class of twenty, you will conclude that<br />

the operation of the laws of chance alone will almost<br />

probably give you close equivalent ‘mixes’ of ‘better’<br />

and ‘poorer’ children in the experimental and control<br />

groups.<br />

Source:adaptedfromPilliner1973<br />

randomization even with a small number of<br />

subjects is well illustrated in Box 13.2.<br />

Randomization, then, ensures the greater<br />

likelihood of equivalence, that is, the apportioning<br />

out between the experimental and control groups<br />

of any other factors or characteristics of the<br />

subjects which might conceivably affect the<br />

experimental variables in which the researcher is<br />

interested. If the groups are made equivalent, then<br />

any so-called ‘clouding’ effects should be present<br />

in both groups.<br />

So strong is this simple and elegant true<br />

experimental design, that all the threats to internal<br />

validity identified in Chapter 6 are, according<br />

to Campbell and Stanley (1963), controlled in<br />

the pretest-post-test control group design. The<br />

causal effect of an intervention can be calculated<br />

in three steps:<br />

1 Subtract the pretest score from the post-test<br />

score for the experimental group to yield<br />

score 1.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!