12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

SYSTEMATIC APPROACHES TO DATA ANALYSIS 469<br />

<br />

assembling and providing sufficient data that<br />

keeps separate raw data from analysis.<br />

In qualitative data the analysis here is almost<br />

inevitably interpretive, hence the data analysis<br />

is less a completely accurate representation (as<br />

in the numerical, positivist tradition) but more<br />

of a reflexive, reactive interaction between the<br />

researcher and the decontextualized data that<br />

are already interpretations of a social encounter.<br />

Indeed reflexivity is an important feature of<br />

qualitative data analysis, and we discuss this<br />

separately (Chapter 7). The issue here is that<br />

the researcher brings to the data his or her<br />

own preconceptions, interests, biases, preferences,<br />

biography, background and agenda. As Walford<br />

(2001: 98) writes: ‘all research is researching<br />

yourself’. In practical terms it means that the<br />

researcher may be selective in his or her focus,<br />

or that the research may be influenced by<br />

the subjective features of the researcher. Robson<br />

(1993: 374–5) and Lincoln and Guba (1985:<br />

354–5) suggest that these can include:<br />

data overload (humans may be unable to<br />

handle large amounts of data)<br />

first impressions (early data analysis may affect<br />

later data collection and analysis)<br />

availability of people (e.g. how representative<br />

these are and how to know if missing people<br />

and data might be important)<br />

information availability (easily accessible<br />

information may receive greater attention than<br />

hard-to-obtain data)<br />

positive instances (researchers may overemphasize<br />

confirming data and underemphasize<br />

disconfirming data).<br />

internal consistency (the unusual, unexpected<br />

or novel may be under-treated).<br />

uneven reliability (the researcher may overlo<strong>ok</strong><br />

the fact that some sources are more reliable or<br />

unreliable than others).<br />

missing data (that issues for which there<br />

is incomplete data may be overlo<strong>ok</strong>ed or<br />

neglected)<br />

revision of hypotheses (researchers may overreact<br />

or under-react to new data)<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

confidence in judgement (researchers may have<br />

greater confidence in their final judgements<br />

than is tenable)<br />

co-occurrence may be mistaken for association<br />

inconsistency (subsequent analyses of the same<br />

data may yield different results); a notable<br />

example of this is Bennett (1976) and Aitkin<br />

et al.(1981).<br />

The issue here is that great caution and selfawareness<br />

must be exercised by the researcher<br />

in conducting qualitative data analysis, for the<br />

analysis and the findings may say more about the<br />

researcher than about the data. For example, it is<br />

the researcher who sets the codes and categories<br />

for analysis, be they pre-ordinate or responsive<br />

(decided in advance of or in response to the data<br />

analysis respectively). It is the researcher’s agenda<br />

that drives the research and the researcher who<br />

chooses the methodology.<br />

As the researcher analyses data, he or she will<br />

have ideas, insights, comments, reflections to make<br />

on data. These can be noted down in memos<br />

and, indeed, these can become data themselves<br />

in the process of reflexivity (though they should<br />

be kept separate from the primary data themselves).<br />

Glaser (1978) and Robson (1993: 387)<br />

argue that memos are not data in themselves<br />

but help the process of data analysis. This is debatable:<br />

if reflexivity is part of the data analysis<br />

process then memos may become legitimate secondary<br />

data in the process or journey of data<br />

analysis. Many computer packages for qualitative<br />

data analysis (discussed later) have a facility not<br />

only for the researcher to write a memo, but also<br />

to attach it to a particular piece of datum. There<br />

is no single nature or format of a memo; it can<br />

include subjective thoughts about the data, with<br />

ideas, theories, reflections, comments, opinions,<br />

personal responses, suggestions for future and new<br />

lines of research, reminders, observations, evaluations,<br />

critiques, judgements, conclusions, explanations,<br />

considerations, implications, speculations,<br />

predictions, hunches, theories, connections, relationships<br />

between codes and categories, insights<br />

and so on. Memos can be reflections on the<br />

past, present and the future, thereby beginning to<br />

Chapter 22

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!