12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

480 CONTENT ANALYSIS AND GROUNDED THEORY<br />

another; they can be logically related; and they<br />

can be empirically related (found to accompany<br />

each other) (Krippendorp 2004: 296).<br />

One has to be aware that the construction<br />

of codes and categories might steer the research<br />

and its findings, i.e. that the researcher may<br />

enter too far into the research process. For<br />

example, a researcher may have been examining<br />

the extra-curricular activities of a school and<br />

discovered that the benefits of these are to be<br />

found in non-cognitive and non-academic spheres<br />

rather than in academic spheres, but this may<br />

be fallacious. It could be that it was the codes<br />

and categories themselves rather than the data<br />

in the minds of the respondents that caused<br />

this separation of cognitive/academic spheres and<br />

issues from the non-cognitive/non-academic, and<br />

that if the researcher had specifically asked about or<br />

established codes and categories which established<br />

the connection between the academic and nonacademic,<br />

then the researcher would have found<br />

more than he or she did. This is the danger of<br />

using codes and categories to predefine the data<br />

analysis.<br />

Step 8: Conduct the coding and<br />

categorizing of the data<br />

Once the codes and categories have been decided,<br />

the analysis can be undertaken. This concerns<br />

the actual ascription of codes and categories to<br />

the text. Coding has been defined by Kerlinger<br />

(1970) as the translation of question responses<br />

and respondent information to specific categories<br />

for the purpose of analysis. As we have seen, many<br />

questions are precoded, that is, each response can<br />

be immediately and directly converted into a score<br />

in an objective way. Rating scales and checklists<br />

are examples of precoded questions. Coding is the<br />

ascription of a category label to a piece of data;<br />

which is either decided in advance or in response<br />

to the data that have been collected.<br />

Mayring (2004: 268–9) suggests that summarizing<br />

content analysis reduces the material to<br />

manageable proportions while maintaining fidelity<br />

to essential contents, and that inductive category<br />

formation proceeds through summarizing content<br />

analysis by inductively generating categories from<br />

the text material. This is in contrast to explicit content<br />

analysis, theoppositeofsummarizingcontent<br />

analysis, which seeks to add in further information<br />

in the search for intelligible text analysis and<br />

category location. The former reduces contextual<br />

detail, the latter retains it. Structuring content<br />

analysis filters out parts of the text in order to construct<br />

a cross-section of the material using specified<br />

pre-ordinate criteria.<br />

It is important to decide whether to code simply<br />

for the existence or the incidence of the concept.<br />

This is important, as it would mean that, in the<br />

case of the former – existence – the frequency of<br />

a concept would be lost, and frequency may give<br />

an indication of the significance of a concept<br />

in the text. Further, the coding will need to<br />

decide whether it should code only the exact<br />

words or those with a similar meaning. The former<br />

will probably result in significant data loss, as<br />

words are not often repeated in comparison to<br />

the concepts that they signify; the latter may<br />

risk losing the nuanced sensitivity of particular<br />

words and phrases. Indeed some speechmakers<br />

may deliberately use ambiguous words or those<br />

with more than one meaning.<br />

In coding a piece of transcription the researcher<br />

goes through the data systematically, typically line<br />

by line, and writes a descriptive code by the side<br />

of each piece of datum, for example:<br />

Text<br />

The students will undertake<br />

problem-solving in science<br />

Iprefertoteachmixedabilityclasses<br />

Code<br />

PROB<br />

MIXABIL<br />

One can see that the codes here are abbreviations,<br />

enabling the researcher to understand immediately<br />

the issue that they denote because they resemble<br />

that issue (rather than, for example, ascribing<br />

a number as a code for each piece of datum,<br />

where the number provides no clue as to what<br />

the datum or category concerns). Where they are<br />

not abbreviations, Miles and Huberman (1994)<br />

suggest that the coding label should bear sufficient<br />

resemblance to the original data so that the<br />

researcher can know, by lo<strong>ok</strong>ing at the code,<br />

what the original piece of datum concerned.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!