12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

280 EXPERIMENTS AND META-ANALYSIS<br />

limits of variability that will be used to define the<br />

matching (e.g. ± 3 points). As before, the greater<br />

the degree of precision in the matching here, the<br />

closer will be the match, but the greater the degree<br />

of precision the harder it will be to find an exactly<br />

matched sample.<br />

One way of addressing this issue is to place all<br />

the subjects in rank order on the basis of the scores<br />

or measures of the dependent variable. Then the<br />

first two subjects become one matched pair (which<br />

one is allocated to the control group and which<br />

to the experimental group is done randomly, e.g.<br />

by tossing a coin), the next two subjects become<br />

the next matched pair, then the next two subjects<br />

become the next matched pair, and so on until<br />

the sample is drawn. Here the loss of precision is<br />

counterbalanced by the avoidance of the loss of<br />

subjects.<br />

The alternative to matching that has been<br />

discussed earlier in the chapter is randomization.<br />

Smith (1991: 215) suggests that matching is<br />

most widely used in quasi-experimental and nonexperimental<br />

research, and is a far inferior means<br />

of ruling out alternative causal explanations than<br />

randomization.<br />

The factorial design<br />

In an experiment there may be two or more<br />

independent variables acting on the dependent<br />

variable. For example, performance in<br />

an examination may be a consequence of<br />

availability of resources (independent variable<br />

one: limited availability, moderate availability,<br />

high availability) and motivation for the subject<br />

studied (independent variable two: little<br />

motivation, moderate motivation, high motivation).<br />

Each independent variable is studied<br />

at each of its levels (in the example<br />

here it is three levels for each independent<br />

variable) (see http://www.routledge.com/<br />

textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/9780415368780 – Chapter 13, file 13.9.<br />

ppt). Participants are randomly assigned to groups<br />

that cover all the possible combinations of levels<br />

of each independent variable, as shown in the<br />

model.<br />

INDEPEN-<br />

DENT<br />

VARIABLE<br />

Availability<br />

of resources<br />

Motivation<br />

for the subject<br />

studied<br />

LEVEL<br />

ONE<br />

Limited<br />

availability<br />

(1)<br />

Little<br />

motivation<br />

(4)<br />

LEVEL<br />

TWO<br />

Moderate<br />

availability<br />

(2)<br />

Moderate<br />

motivation<br />

(5)<br />

LEVEL<br />

THREE<br />

High<br />

availability<br />

(3)<br />

High<br />

motivation<br />

(6)<br />

Here the possible combinations are: 1 + 4,<br />

1 + 5, 1 + 6, 2 + 4, 2 + 5, 2 + 6, 3 + 4, 3 + 5and<br />

3 + 6. This yields 9 groups (3 × 3combinations).<br />

Pretests and post-tests or post-tests only can<br />

be conducted. It might show, for example,<br />

that limited availability of resources and little<br />

motivation had a statistically significant influence<br />

on examination performance, whereas moderate<br />

and high availability of resources did not, or<br />

that high availability and high motivation had<br />

a statistically significant effect on performance,<br />

whereas high motivation and limited availability<br />

did not, and so on.<br />

This example assumes that there are the same<br />

number of levels for each independent variable;<br />

this may not be the case. One variable may have,<br />

say, two levels, another three levels, and another<br />

four levels. Here the possible combinations are<br />

2 × 3 × 4 = 24 levels and, therefore, 24 experimental<br />

groups. One can see that factorial designs<br />

quickly generate several groups of participants. A<br />

common example is a 2 × 2design,inwhichtwo<br />

independent variables each have two values (i.e.<br />

four groups). Here experimental group 1 receives<br />

the intervention with independent variable 1 at<br />

level 1 and independent variable 2 at level 1; experimental<br />

group 2 receives the intervention with<br />

independent variable 1 at level 1 and independent<br />

variable 2 at level 2; experimental group 3 receives<br />

the intervention with independent variable 1 at<br />

level 2 and independent variable 2 at level 1; experimental<br />

group 4 receives the intervention with<br />

independent variable 1 at level 2 and independent<br />

variable 2 at level 2.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!