12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

404 OBSERVATION<br />

Critical incidents<br />

There will be times when reliability as consistency<br />

in observations is not always necessary. For<br />

example, a student might demonstrate a particular<br />

behaviour only once, but it is so important as<br />

not to be ruled out simply because it occurred<br />

once. One has to commit only a single murder<br />

to be branded a murderer! Sometimes one<br />

event can occur which reveals an extremely<br />

important insight into a person or situation.<br />

Critical incidents (Flanagan 1949) and critical<br />

events (Wragg 1994) are particular events or<br />

occurrences that might typify or illuminate very<br />

starkly a particular feature of a teacher’s behaviour<br />

or teaching style for example. Wragg (1994: 64)<br />

writes that these are events that appear to the<br />

observer to have more interest than other ones,<br />

and therefore warrant greater detail and recording<br />

than other events; they have an important insight<br />

to offer. For example, a child might unexpectedly<br />

behave very aggressively when asked to work<br />

with another child – that might reveal an insight<br />

into the child’s social tolerance; a teacher might<br />

suddenly overreact when a student produces a<br />

substandard piece of work – the straw that breaks<br />

the camel’s back – that might indicate a level of<br />

frustration tolerance or intolerance and the effects<br />

of that threshold of tolerance being reached.<br />

These events are critical in that they may be<br />

non-routine but very revealing; they offer the<br />

researcher an insight that would not be available by<br />

routine observation. They are frequently unusual<br />

events. 2<br />

Naturalistic and participant observation<br />

There are degrees of participation in observation<br />

(LeCompte and Preissle 1993: 93–4). The<br />

‘complete participant’ is a researcher who takes<br />

on an insider role in the group being studied, and<br />

maybe who does not even declare that he or she<br />

is a researcher (discussed later in comments about<br />

the ethics of covert research). The ‘participant-asobserver’,<br />

as its name suggests, is part of the social<br />

life of participants and documents and records<br />

what is happening for research purposes. The<br />

‘observer-as-participant’, like the participant-asobserver,<br />

is known as a researcher to the group,<br />

and maybe has less extensive contact with the<br />

group. With the ‘complete observer’ participants<br />

do not realize that they are being observed<br />

(e.g. using a one-way mirror), hence this is<br />

another form of covert research. Hammersley and<br />

Atkinson (1983: 93–5) suggest that comparative<br />

involvement may come in the forms of the<br />

complete participant and the participant-asobserver,<br />

with a degree of subjectivity and<br />

sympathy, while comparative detachment may<br />

come in the forms of the observer-as-participant<br />

and the complete observer, where objectivity and<br />

distance are key characteristics. Both complete<br />

participation and complete detachment are as<br />

limiting as each other. As a complete participant<br />

the researcher dare not go outside the confines of<br />

the group for fear of revealing his or her identity (in<br />

covert research), and as a complete observer there<br />

is no contact with the observed, so inference is<br />

dangerous. That said, both complete participation<br />

and complete detachment minimize reactivity,<br />

though in the former there is the risk of ‘going<br />

native’ – where the researcher adopts the values,<br />

norms and behaviours of the group, i.e. ceases to be<br />

aresearcherandbecomesamemberofthegroup.<br />

Participant observation may be particularly<br />

useful in studying small groups, or for events<br />

and processes that last only a short time or are<br />

frequent, for activities that lend themselves to<br />

being observed, for researchers who wish to reach<br />

inside a situation and have a long time available<br />

to them to ‘get under the skin’ of behaviour<br />

or organizations (as in an ethnography), and<br />

when the prime interest is in gathering detailed<br />

information about what is happening (i.e. is<br />

descriptive).<br />

In participant observational studies the researcher<br />

stays with the participants for a substantial<br />

period of time to reduce reactivity effects (the effects<br />

of the researcher on the researched, changing<br />

the behaviour of the latter), recording what is<br />

happening, while taking a role in that situation.<br />

In schools this might be taking on some particular<br />

activities, sharing supervisions, participating in<br />

school life, recording impressions, conversations,

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!