12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

TRUE EXPERIMENTAL DESIGNS 279<br />

textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/9780415368780 – Chapter 13, file 13.8.<br />

ppt). So, for example, the designs might be:<br />

Experimental 1 RO 1 X 1 O 2<br />

Experimental 2 RO 3 X 2 O 4<br />

Control RO 5 O 6<br />

This can be extended to the post-test control and<br />

experimental group design and the post-test two<br />

experimental groups design, and the pretest-posttest<br />

two treatment design.<br />

The matched pairs design<br />

As the name suggests, here participants are<br />

allocated to control and experimental groups<br />

randomly, but the basis of the allocation is that<br />

one member of the control group is matched to a<br />

member of the experimental group on the several<br />

independent variables considered important for<br />

the study (e.g. those independent variables that are<br />

considered to have an influence on the dependent<br />

variable, such as sex, age, ability). So, first, pairs of<br />

participants are selected who are matched in terms<br />

of the independent variable under consideration<br />

(e.g. whose scores on a particular measure are the<br />

same or similar), and then each of the pair is<br />

randomly assigned to the control or experimental<br />

group. Randomization takes place at the pair<br />

rather than the group level. Although, as its name<br />

suggests, this ensures effective matching of control<br />

and experimental groups, in practice it may not be<br />

easy to find sufficiently close matching, particularly<br />

in a field experiment, although finding such a<br />

close match in a field experiment may increase the<br />

control of the experiment considerably. Matched<br />

pairs designs are useful if the researcher cannot be<br />

certain that individual differences will not obscure<br />

treatment effects, as it enables these individual<br />

differences to be controlled.<br />

Borg and Gall (1979: 547) set out a useful<br />

series of steps in the planning and conduct of an<br />

experiment:<br />

1 Carryoutameasureofthedependentvariable.<br />

2 Assign participants to matched pairs, based<br />

on the scores and measures established from<br />

Step 1.<br />

3 Randomly assign one person from each pair<br />

to the control group and the other to the<br />

experimental group.<br />

4 Administer the experimental treatment/<br />

intervention to the experimental group and,<br />

if appropriate, a placebo to the control group.<br />

Ensure that the control group is not subject<br />

to the intervention.<br />

5 Carryoutameasureofthedependentvariable<br />

with both groups and compare/measure them<br />

in order to determine the effect and its size on<br />

the dependent variable.<br />

Borg and Gall indicate that difficulties arise<br />

in the close matching of the sample of the<br />

control and experimental groups. This involves<br />

careful identification of the variables on which<br />

the matching must take place. Borg and Gall<br />

(1979: 547) suggest that matching on a number<br />

of variables that correlate with the dependent<br />

variable is more likely to reduce errors than<br />

matching on a single variable. The problem, of<br />

course, is that the greater the number of variables<br />

that have to be matched, the harder it is actually<br />

to find the sample of people who are matched.<br />

Hence the balance must be struck between having<br />

too few variables such that error can occur, and<br />

having so many variables that it is impossible<br />

to draw a sample. Instead of matched pairs,<br />

random allocation is possible, and this is discussed<br />

below.<br />

Mitchell and Jolley (1988: 103) pose three<br />

important questions that researchers need to<br />

consider when comparing two groups:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

Are the two groups equal at the commencement<br />

of the experiment<br />

Would the two groups have grown apart<br />

naturally, regardless of the intervention<br />

To what extent has initial measurement error<br />

of the two groups been a contributory factor in<br />

differences between scores<br />

Borg and Gall (1979) draw attention to the need<br />

to specify the degree of exactitude (or variance)<br />

of the match. For example, if the subjects were to<br />

be matched on, say, linguistic ability as measured<br />

in a standardized test, it is important to define the<br />

Chapter 13

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!