12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

NATURALISTIC AND PARTICIPANT OBSERVATION 405<br />

observations, comments, behaviour, events and<br />

activities and the views of all participants in a situation.<br />

Participant observation is often combined<br />

with other forms of data collection that, together,<br />

elicit the participants’ definitions of the situation<br />

and their organizing constructs in accounting for<br />

situations and behaviour. By staying in a situation<br />

over a long period the researcher is also<br />

able to see how events evolve over time, catching<br />

the dynamics of situations, the people, personalities,<br />

contexts, resources, roles etc. Morrison (1993:<br />

88) argues that by ‘being immersed in a particular<br />

context over time not only will the salient<br />

features of the situation emerge and present themselves<br />

but a more holistic view will be gathered of<br />

the interrelationships of factors’. Such immersion<br />

facilitates the generation of ‘thick descriptions’,<br />

particularly of social processes and interaction,<br />

which lend themselves to accurate explanation<br />

and interpretation of events rather than relying<br />

on the researcher’s own inferences. The data derived<br />

from participant observation are ‘strong on<br />

reality’.<br />

Components of ‘thick descriptions’ involve<br />

recording (Carspecken 1996: 47), for example:<br />

speech acts; non-verbal communication; descriptions<br />

in low-inference vocabulary; careful and<br />

frequent recording of the time and timing of<br />

events; the observer’s comments that are placed<br />

into categories; detailed contextual data.<br />

Observations are recorded in field notes; these<br />

can be written at several levels (see http://<br />

www.routledge.com/textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/9780415368780 –<br />

Chapter 18, file 18.8.ppt). At the level of description<br />

(Spradley 1980; Bogdan and Biklen 1992:<br />

120–1; LeCompte and Preissle 1993: 224), observations<br />

can take the following forms:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

quick, fragmentary jottings of key words/<br />

symbols<br />

transcriptions and more detailed observations<br />

written out fully<br />

descriptions that, when assembled and written<br />

out, form a comprehensive and comprehensible<br />

account of what has happened<br />

pen portraits of participants<br />

reconstructions of conversations<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

descriptions of the physical settings of events<br />

descriptions of events, behaviour and activities<br />

adescriptionoftheresearcher’sactivitiesand<br />

behaviour.<br />

Lincoln and Guba (1985: 273) suggest a variety<br />

of elements or types of observations that include:<br />

ongoing notes, either verbatim or categorized<br />

in situ<br />

logs or diaries of field experiences (similar to<br />

field notes though usually written some time<br />

after the observations have been made)<br />

notes that are made on specific, predetermined<br />

themes (e.g. that have arisen from grounded<br />

theory)<br />

‘chronologs’, where each separate behavioural<br />

episode is noted, together with the time at<br />

which it occurred, or recording an observation<br />

at regular time intervals, e.g. every two or three<br />

minutes<br />

context maps: maps, sketches, diagrams or<br />

some graphic display of the context (usually<br />

physical) within which the observation takes<br />

place, such graphics enabling movements to be<br />

charted<br />

entries on predetermined schedules (including<br />

rating scales, checklists and structured observation<br />

charts), using taxonomic or categoric<br />

systems, where the categories derive from previous<br />

observational or interview data<br />

sociometric diagrams indicating social relationships,<br />

e.g. isolates (whom nobody chooses),<br />

stars (whom everyone chooses) and dyads (who<br />

choose each other)<br />

debriefing questionnaires from respondents<br />

that are devised for, and by, the observer only,<br />

to be used for reminding the observer of main<br />

types of information and events once she or he<br />

has left the scene<br />

data from debriefing sessions with other<br />

researchers, again as an aide-memoire.<br />

LeCompte and Preissle (1993: 199–200)<br />

provide a useful set of guidelines for directing<br />

observations of specific activities, events or scenes,<br />

suggesting that they should include answers to the<br />

following questions:<br />

Chapter 18

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!