12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

222 SURVEYS AND DEVELOPMENTAL STUDIES<br />

the interviewer to travel long distances to reach<br />

interviewees, which can be expensive both in time<br />

and travel costs (Bailey 1994: 175). If interviews<br />

are intended to be conducted in the participants’<br />

own homes, then participants may be unwilling to<br />

admit strangers. Moreover, neighbourhoods may<br />

be dangerous for some researchers to visit (e.g.<br />

awhiteresearcherwithaclipboardgoingintoa<br />

non-white area of great deprivation, or a black<br />

researcher going into a conservative white area).<br />

Telephone surveys<br />

Telephone surveys, it is claimed (Dooley 2001:<br />

122), have the advantage of reducing costs in time<br />

and travel, for where a potential respondent is not<br />

at home a call-back costs only a few coins and the<br />

time to redial. Re-visits to often distant locations,<br />

on the other hand, can incur considerable expense<br />

in time and travel. Furthermore, if the intended<br />

participant is unable or unwilling to respond,<br />

then it is a relatively easy matter to maintain<br />

the required sample size by calling a replacement.<br />

Again, where respondents are unable or unwilling<br />

to answer all the questions required, then their<br />

partial replies may be discarded and further<br />

substitutes sought from the sample listing. It is<br />

easy to see why telephone interviews must always<br />

have a much longer list of potential respondents<br />

in order to attain the required sample size.<br />

On the other hand, not everyone has a<br />

telephone (e.g. the poor, the young, the less<br />

educated) and this may lead to a skewed sample.<br />

Nor, for that matter, is everyone available for<br />

interview, particularly if they work. Furthermore,<br />

many people are ex-directory, i.e. their numbers<br />

are withheld from public scrutiny. In addition,<br />

Dooley (2001: 123) reports that others – the<br />

younger, unmarried and higher occupational status<br />

groups – use answering machines that may screen<br />

out and delete researchers’ calls. These could also<br />

lead to a skewed sample.<br />

Even when the telephone is answered, the<br />

person responding may not be the most suitable<br />

one to take the call; she or he may not know<br />

the answer to the questions or have access to the<br />

kind of information required. For example, in an<br />

inquiry about household budgets, the respondent<br />

may simply be ignorant about a family’s income<br />

or expenditure on particular items. A child may<br />

answer the call or an elderly person who may<br />

not be the householder. Interviewers will need to<br />

prepare a set of preliminary, screening questions or<br />

arrange a call-back time when a more appropriate<br />

person can be interviewed.<br />

Telephone interviewing has its own strengths<br />

and weaknesses. For example, more often than not<br />

arespondent’ssexwillbeclearfromtheirvoice,so<br />

particular questions may be inappropriate. On the<br />

other hand, it is unwise to have several multiple<br />

choices in a telephone interview, as respondents<br />

will simply forget the categories available, there<br />

being no written prompts to which the respondent<br />

can refer.<br />

Similarly, order effects can be high: items<br />

appearing early in the interview exert an influence<br />

on responses to later ones, while items appearing<br />

early in a list of responses may be given<br />

greater consideration than those occurring later,<br />

amatternotconfinedtotelephonesurveysbut<br />

to questionnaires in general. Dooley (2001: 136)<br />

indicates that 17 per cent difference in agreement<br />

was recorded to a general statement question<br />

when it appeared before rather than after aspecific<br />

statement. He cites further research demonstrating<br />

that responses to particular questions are affected<br />

by questions surrounding them. His advice is to ask<br />

general questions before specific ones. Otherwise,<br />

the general questions are influenced by earlier<br />

responses to specific questions. Once again, this is<br />

amatternotconfinedtotelephonesurveysbutto<br />

questionnaires in general.<br />

Further, if the questioning becomes too<br />

sensitive, respondents may simply hang up in the<br />

middle of the survey interview, tell lies or withhold<br />

information. Dooley (2001: 123) reports that, in<br />

comparison to face-to-face interviews, telephone<br />

respondents tend to produce more missing data, to<br />

be more evasive, more acquiescent (i.e. they tend<br />

to agree more with statements) and more extreme<br />

in their responses (e.g. opting for the extreme ends<br />

of rating scales).<br />

Because telephone interviews lack the sensory<br />

stimulation of visual or face-to-face interviews

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!