12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

324 QUESTIONNAIRES<br />

the researcher may be asking which post-16 course<br />

a student is following (e.g. academic, vocational,<br />

manual, non-manual). In these cases only one<br />

response may be selected. As with the dichotomous<br />

variable, the listing of several categories or<br />

elements of a variable (e.g. form membership<br />

and course followed) enables nominal data to<br />

be collected and processed using the chi-square<br />

statistic, the G-test and cross-tabulations (Cohen<br />

and Holliday 1996). Multiple choice questions are<br />

treated as nominal data (see Part Five).<br />

It may be important to include in the multiple<br />

choices those that will enable respondents to<br />

select the response that most closely represents<br />

their view, hence a pilot is needed to ensure that<br />

the categories are comprehensive, exhaustive and<br />

representative. On the other hand, the researcher<br />

may be interested in certain features only, and it is<br />

these that would figure in the response categories<br />

only.<br />

The multiple choice questionnaire seldom gives<br />

more than a crude statistic, for words are inherently<br />

ambiguous. In the example above of chemistry, the<br />

notion of ‘useful’ is unclear, as are ‘appropriate’,<br />

‘practicable’ and ‘burden’. Respondents could<br />

interpret these words differently in their own<br />

contexts, thereby rendering the data ambiguous.<br />

One respondent might see the utility of the<br />

chemistry scheme in one area and thereby say<br />

that it is useful – ticking (d). Another respondent<br />

might see the same utility in that same one area<br />

but, because it is only useful in that single area,<br />

may see this as a flaw and therefore not tick<br />

category (d). With an anonymous questionnaire<br />

this difference would be impossible to detect.<br />

This is the heart of the problem of questionnaires<br />

– that different respondents interpret the<br />

same words differently. ‘Anchor statements’ can<br />

be provided to allow a degree of discrimination<br />

in response (e.g. ‘strongly agree’, ‘agree’ etc.) but<br />

there is no guarantee that respondents will always<br />

interpret them in the way that is intended. In<br />

the example above this might not be a problem<br />

as the researcher might only be seeking an index<br />

of utility – without wishing to know the areas of<br />

utility or the reasons for that utility. The evaluator<br />

might be wishing only for a crude statistic (which<br />

might be very useful statistically in making a decisive<br />

judgement about a programme). In this case<br />

this rough and ready statistic might be perfectly<br />

acceptable.<br />

One can see in the example of chemistry<br />

above not only ambiguity in the wording but<br />

also a very incomplete set of response categories<br />

which is hardly capable of representing all aspects<br />

of the chemistry scheme. That this might be<br />

politically expedient cannot be overlo<strong>ok</strong>ed, for<br />

if the choice of responses is limited, then those<br />

responses might enable bias to be built into the<br />

research. For example, if the responses were limited<br />

to statements about the utility of the chemistry<br />

scheme, then the evaluator would have little<br />

difficulty in establishing that the scheme was<br />

useful. By avoiding the inclusion of negative<br />

statements or the opportunity to record a negative<br />

response the research will surely be biased. The<br />

issue of the wording of questions has been discussed<br />

earlier.<br />

Multiple choice items are also prone to<br />

problems of word order and statement order. For<br />

example, Dillman et al.(2003:6)reportastudyof<br />

German students who were asked to compare their<br />

high school teachers in terms of whether male<br />

or female teachers were more empathetic. They<br />

found that respondents rated their female teachers<br />

more highly than their male teachers when asked<br />

to compare female teachers to male teachers than<br />

when they were asked to compare their male<br />

teachers to their female teachers. Similarly they<br />

report a study in which tennis was found to be less<br />

exciting than football when the tennis option was<br />

presented before the football option, and more<br />

exciting when the football option was placed<br />

before the tennis option. These studies suggest<br />

that respondents tend to judge later items in terms<br />

of the earlier items, rather than vice versa and<br />

that they overlo<strong>ok</strong> features specific to later items<br />

if these are not contained in the earlier items.<br />

This is an instance of the ‘primacy effect’ or ‘order<br />

effect’, wherein items earlier in a list are given<br />

greater weight than items lower in the list. Order<br />

effects are resilient to efforts to minimize them, and<br />

primacy effects are particularly strong in Internet<br />

questionnaires (Dillman et al. 2003: 22).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!