12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

400 OBSERVATION<br />

schedule itself. So, for example, if close, detailed<br />

scrutiny is required then the time intervals will be<br />

very short, and if less detail is required then the<br />

intervals may be longer.<br />

Dyer (1995: 181–4) suggests that structured<br />

observation must address several key principles:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

The choice of the environment, such that<br />

there will be opportunities for the behaviour<br />

to be observed to be actually occurring – the<br />

availability and frequency of the behaviour of<br />

interest to the observer: a key feature if unusual<br />

or special behaviour is sought.<br />

The need for clear and unambiguous measures,<br />

particularly if a latent characteristic or<br />

construct is being operationalized.<br />

A manageable number of variables: a sufficient<br />

number for validity to be demonstrated, yet not<br />

so many as to render data entry unreliable.<br />

Overt or covert observation.<br />

Continuous, time-series or random observation.<br />

The different categories of behaviour to be<br />

observed.<br />

The number of people to be observed.<br />

The number of variables on which data must<br />

be gathered.<br />

The kind of observation schedule to be used.<br />

Dyer (1995: 186) provides a checklist for planning<br />

a structured observation (Box 18.2).<br />

There are five principal ways of entering<br />

data onto a structured observation schedule:<br />

event sampling, instantaneous sampling, interval<br />

recording, rating scales and duration recording.<br />

Event sampling<br />

Event sampling, also known as a sign system,<br />

requires a tally mark to be entered against each<br />

statement each time it is observed, for example (see<br />

http://www.routledge.com/textbo<strong>ok</strong>s/<br />

9780415368780 – Chapter 18, file 18.4.ppt):<br />

teacher shouts at child /////<br />

child shouts at teacher ///<br />

parent shouts at teacher //<br />

teacher shouts at parent //<br />

The researcher will need to devise statements<br />

that yield the data that answer the research<br />

questions. This method is useful for finding out<br />

the frequencies or incidence of observed situations<br />

or behaviours, so that comparisons can be made;<br />

we can tell, for example, that the teacher does most<br />

shouting and that the parent shouts least of all.<br />

However, while these data enable us to chart the<br />

incidence of observed situations or behaviours,<br />

the difficulty with them is that we are unable<br />

to determine the chronological order in which<br />

they occurred. For example, two different stories<br />

could be told from these data if the sequence of<br />

events were known. If the data were presented in<br />

achronology,onestorycouldbeseenasfollows,<br />

where the numbers 1–7 are the different periods<br />

over time (e.g. every thirty seconds):<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

teacher shouts at child / / / / /<br />

child shouts at teacher / / /<br />

parent shouts at teacher / /<br />

teacher shouts at parent / /<br />

Imagine the scene: a parent and child arrive late<br />

for school one morning and the child slips into the<br />

classroom; an event quickly occurs which prompts<br />

the child to shout at the teacher, the exasperated<br />

teacher is very cross when thus prov<strong>ok</strong>ed by the<br />

child; the teacher shouts at the child, who then<br />

brings in the parent (who has not yet left the<br />

premises); the parent shouts at the teacher for<br />

unreasonable behaviour and the teacher shouts<br />

back at the child. It seems in this version that the<br />

teacher shouts only when prov<strong>ok</strong>ed by the child<br />

or parent.<br />

If the same number of tally marks were<br />

distributed in a different order, a very different<br />

story might emerge, for example:<br />

1 2 3 4 5 6 7<br />

teacher shouts at child / / / / /<br />

child shouts at teacher / / /<br />

parent shouts at teacher / /<br />

teacher shouts at parent / /<br />

In this scene it is the teacher who is the instigator<br />

of the shouting, shouting at the child and<br />

then at the parent; the child and the parent

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!