12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

TRIANGULATION 141<br />

student’s problem-solving ability. The researcher<br />

might observe the student working on a problem,<br />

or might talk to the student about how she is<br />

tackling the problem, or might ask the student<br />

to write down how she tackled the problem.<br />

Here the researcher has three different datacollecting<br />

instruments – observation, interview<br />

and documentation respectively. If the results<br />

all agreed – concurred – that, according to given<br />

criteria for problem-solving ability, the student<br />

demonstrated a good ability to solve a problem,<br />

then the researcher would be able to say with<br />

greater confidence (validity) that the student was<br />

good at problem-solving than if the researcher had<br />

arrived at that judgement simply from using one<br />

instrument.<br />

Concurrent validity is very similar to its<br />

partner – predictive validity – in its core concept<br />

(i.e. agreement with a second measure); what<br />

differentiates concurrent and predictive validity<br />

is the absence of a time element in the former;<br />

concurrence can be demonstrated simultaneously<br />

with another instrument.<br />

An important partner to concurrent validity,<br />

which is also a bridge into later discussions of<br />

reliability, is triangulation.<br />

Triangulation<br />

Triangulation may be defined as the use of two<br />

or more methods of data collection in the study<br />

of some aspect of human behaviour. The use of<br />

multiple methods, or the multi-method approach<br />

as it is sometimes called, contrasts with the<br />

ubiquitous but generally more vulnerable singlemethod<br />

approach that characterizes so much of<br />

research in the social sciences. In its original<br />

and literal sense, triangulation is a technique<br />

of physical measurement: maritime navigators,<br />

military strategists and surveyors, for example,<br />

use (or used to use) several locational markers<br />

in their endeavours to pinpoint a single spot<br />

or objective. By analogy, triangular techniques<br />

in the social sciences attempt to map out, or<br />

explain more fully, the richness and complexity<br />

of human behaviour by studying it from more<br />

than one standpoint and, in so doing, by making<br />

use of both quantitative and qualitative data.<br />

Triangulation is a powerful way of demonstrating<br />

concurrent validity, particularly in qualitative<br />

research (Campbell and Fiske 1959).<br />

The advantages of the multi-method approach<br />

in social research are manifold and we examine<br />

two of them. First, whereas the single observation<br />

in fields such as medicine, chemistry and<br />

physics normally yields sufficient and unambiguous<br />

information on selected phenomena, it provides<br />

only a limited view of the complexity of human<br />

behaviour and of situations in which human<br />

beings interact. It has been observed that as<br />

research methods act as filters through which<br />

the environment is selectively experienced, they<br />

are never atheoretical or neutral in representing<br />

the world of experience (Smith 1975). Exclusive<br />

reliance on one method, therefore, may bias or<br />

distort the researcher’s picture of the particular<br />

slice of reality being investigated. The researcher<br />

needs to be confident that the data generated<br />

are not simply artefacts of one specific method<br />

of collection (Lin 1976). Such confidence can<br />

be achieved, as far as nomothetic research<br />

is concerned, when different methods of data<br />

collection yield substantially the same results.<br />

(Where triangulation is used in interpretive<br />

research to investigate different actors’ viewpoints,<br />

the same method, e.g. accounts, will naturally<br />

produce different sets of data.)<br />

Further, the more the methods contrast with<br />

each other, the greater the researcher’s confidence.<br />

If, for example, the outcomes of a questionnaire<br />

survey correspond to those of an observational<br />

study of the same phenomena, the more the<br />

researcher will be confident about the findings.<br />

Or, more extreme, where the results of a rigorous<br />

experimental investigation are replicated in,<br />

say, a role-playing exercise, the researcher will<br />

experience even greater assurance. If findings are<br />

artefacts of method, then the use of contrasting<br />

methods considerably reduces the chances of<br />

any consistent findings being attributable to<br />

similarities of method (Lin 1976).<br />

We come now to a second advantage: some<br />

theorists have been sharply critical of the limited<br />

use to which existing methods of inquiry in the<br />

Chapter 6

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!