12.01.2015 Views

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

RESEARCH METHOD COHEN ok

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

164 VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY<br />

operationalization of an unseen, half-guessed-at<br />

construct or phenomenon. The community of<br />

scholars has a role to play here. For a full discussion<br />

of validity see Messick (1993). To conclude this<br />

chapter, we turn briefly to consider validity and<br />

reliability in life history accounts.<br />

Validity and reliability in life histories<br />

Three central issues underpin the quality of data<br />

generated by life history methodology. They are to<br />

do with representativeness, validity and reliability.<br />

Plummer draws attention to a frequent criticism<br />

of life history research, namely that its cases<br />

are atypical rather than representative. To avoid<br />

this charge, he urges intending researchers to,<br />

‘work out and explicitly state the life history’s<br />

relationship to a wider population’ (Plummer<br />

1983) by way of appraising the subject on<br />

a continuum of representativeness and nonrepresentativeness.<br />

Reliability in life history research hinges upon<br />

the identification of sources of bias and the<br />

application of techniques to reduce them. Bias<br />

arises from the informant, the researcher, and the<br />

interactional encounter itself. Box 6.1, adapted<br />

from Plummer (1983), provides a checklist of some<br />

aspects of bias arising from these principal sources.<br />

Several validity checks are available to<br />

intending researchers. Plummer (1983) identifies<br />

the following:<br />

<br />

<br />

<br />

The subject of the life history may present<br />

an autocritique of it, having read the entire<br />

product.<br />

A comparison may be made with similar<br />

written sources by way of identifying points<br />

of major divergence or similarity.<br />

Acomparisonmaybemadewithofficialrecords<br />

by way of imposing accuracy checks on the life<br />

history.<br />

Box 6.1<br />

Principal sources of bias in life history research<br />

Source: Informant<br />

Is misinformation (unintended) given<br />

Has there been evasion<br />

Is there evidence of direct lying and deception<br />

Is a ‘front’ being presented<br />

What may the informant ‘take for granted’ and<br />

hence not reveal<br />

How far is the informant ‘pleasing you’<br />

How much has been forgotten<br />

How much may be self-deception<br />

Source: Researcher<br />

Attitudes of researcher: age, gender, class, race,<br />

religion, politics etc.<br />

Demeanour of researcher: dress, speech, body<br />

language etc.<br />

Personality of researcher: anxiety, need for<br />

approval, hostility, warmth etc.<br />

Scientific role of researcher: theory held (etc.),<br />

researcher expectancy<br />

Source: The interaction<br />

The encounter needs to be examined. Is bias<br />

coming from:<br />

The physical setting – ‘social space’<br />

The prior interaction<br />

Non-verbal communication<br />

Vocal behaviour<br />

Source: adapted from Plummer 1983: Table 5.2, p. 103<br />

<br />

Acomparisonmaybemadebyinterviewing<br />

other informants.<br />

Essentially, the validity of any life history lies in<br />

its ability to represent the informant’s subjective<br />

reality, that is to say, his or her definition of the<br />

situation.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!