02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

MENTAL MODELS 87<br />

Some B are A.<br />

All B are C.<br />

many subjects say, “No valid conclusion,” even though “Some B are A” means the<br />

same as “Some A are B.” Those subjects who are correct in the second form are<br />

also more likely to state the conclusion as “Some C are A,” possibly because they<br />

have switched around the “arrows” in their mental model of the second premise. The<br />

figure seems to affect both the difficulty of the problem <strong>and</strong> the way in which the<br />

conclusion is expressed.<br />

Ability to solve categorical syllogisms is affected both by the tendency to consider<br />

alternative models <strong>and</strong> by the figural effect. Subjects who have the greatest<br />

difficulty are often unable to put together even a single model, especially when the<br />

premises are not in the most convenient order (that is, A–B, B–C). These subjects<br />

find the problem particularly hard when one of the premises must be turned around.<br />

Other subjects do not find the problem hard when only a single model must be considered,<br />

almost regardless of the figure, but do have trouble with multiple-model<br />

problems.<br />

Galotti, Baron, <strong>and</strong> Sabini (1986) found support for this analysis. Subjects were<br />

selected according to their scores on a test of syllogistic reasoning. Subjects were<br />

asked to give an initial answer under time pressure, <strong>and</strong> a final answer without pressure.<br />

It was thought that the initial answer might be based on a single model of<br />

both premises, <strong>and</strong> that the final answer in good reasoners (high scorers) would result<br />

from consideration of alternative models inconsistent with the initial conclusion.<br />

Good reasoners were in fact more likely to correct answers that were initially incorrect,<br />

especially on problems in which “nothing follows,” which always involved<br />

more than a single model. Good reasoners also took about twice as much time as<br />

poor reasoners between initial <strong>and</strong> final answers. (The two groups were about equal<br />

in the time spent on initial answers.) Apparently, those who were good at the task<br />

were more thorough in their search, <strong>and</strong> possibly more self-critical as well. They<br />

seemed (in think-aloud tasks) to search for models that were inconsistent with their<br />

initial conclusion.<br />

The mental-model approach can be extended to both predicate logic <strong>and</strong> propositional<br />

logic. Consider the following examples of predicate logic:<br />

1. None of the As is in the same place as any of the Bs.<br />

All of the Bs are in the same place as all of the Cs.<br />

2. None of the As is in the same place as any of the Bs.<br />

All of the Bs are in the same place as some of the Cs.<br />

Problem 1 easily leads to the conclusion “None of the As is in the same place as any<br />

of the Cs.” A single model is required for this: AAA / BBBCCC. (The slash separates<br />

the different places.) If a single model of the second problem is constructed, an<br />

incorrect conclusion might be drawn. Specifically, the model AAA / BBBCC / CC<br />

might yield the conclusion “None of the As is in the same place as any of the Cs.” The<br />

correct answer requires consideration of a second model, AAACC / BBBCC. The

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!