02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

ACCURACY OF PROBABILITY JUDGMENTS 143<br />

Is it humanly possible to be well calibrated? Do errors result of an inherent<br />

limitation on our accuracy? We do not know the answer to this in general. We do<br />

know the answer for expert judges who make probability judgments every day as<br />

part of their work. The answer is that such people can be amazingly well calibrated.<br />

Murphy <strong>and</strong> Winkler (1977) studied the calibration of some 25,000 weather forecasts<br />

made in the Chicago area over a four-year period ending in 1976. Since 1965,<br />

weather forecasts have included probability information for rain, snow, <strong>and</strong> other<br />

conditions. Weather forecasters use a variety of information, including statistical<br />

tables of events of past years, but they do make their predictions on the basis of personal<br />

judgment; weather forecasts are made by people, not computers. Murphy <strong>and</strong><br />

Winkler found that calibration was nearly perfect. When the forecasters said that<br />

there was a 100% chance of rain, it rained about 98% of the time, <strong>and</strong> so on down<br />

the line.<br />

This does not mean that the forecasts were always very informative. Again, if<br />

you know that it rains on one out of every ten days, your forecasts can be perfectly<br />

calibrated if you give .10 as the probability of rain, every day. Of course, this is not<br />

what the forecasters did.<br />

Most important, this result shows that inappropriate extreme overconfidence can<br />

be overcome. Whatever their accuracy in predicting the weather itself, weather forecasters<br />

are extremely accurate in assessing the confidence that should be placed in<br />

their own predictions. Quite possibly they learn this from years of feedback.<br />

Another cause for the phenomenon of “overconfidence when confidence is high”<br />

(aside from regression to the mean) is a bias that I have emphasized earlier: the tendency<br />

to seek evidence in favor of an initial belief, as opposed to evidence against<br />

it. Koriat, Lichtenstein, <strong>and</strong> Fischhoff (1980) gave subjects the same sort of twoalternative<br />

questions used in the studies described earlier <strong>and</strong> asked for confidence<br />

judgments. Some subjects were asked to give reasons for <strong>and</strong> against their favored<br />

answer before assigning a confidence judgment. Other subjects gave only for reasons,<br />

others against reasons, <strong>and</strong> others none at all (as in the original studies). The<br />

overconfidence phenomenon was reduced (but not completely eliminated) in those<br />

subjects who were asked for both for <strong>and</strong> against reasons <strong>and</strong> in those subjects who<br />

were asked for against reasons alone. Apparently subjects were failing to think of<br />

such criticisms on their own, without the explicit instruction to do so. Subjects who<br />

were asked to give for reasons did not differ from the control group that gave no reasons<br />

at all. Apparently subjects think of for reasons on their own, without prompting.<br />

In a related study, Hoch (1985) asked graduating business students, just beginning<br />

their search for jobs, to assign probabilities to various outcomes of their job<br />

search, such as “What is the probability that your starting salary will exceed $ ?”<br />

or “What is the probability that you will receive more than job offers by the end<br />

of the school year?” Actual data were collected on the same subjects at the end of the<br />

school year from computer records of the placement office. Subjects generally had<br />

been overconfident, although they were more overconfident when asked about high<br />

salaries as opposed to low ones, <strong>and</strong> about few offers as opposed to many. Subjects<br />

who were told to think of reasons why the event in question might not occur showed

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!