02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

194 JUDGMENT OF CORRELATION AND CONTINGENCY<br />

the teacher expects such a correlation, she tends to perceive it in children’s behavior.<br />

The correlations that people expect to see, however, are not always the ones that are<br />

present.<br />

This was demonstrated in a study done by Shweder (1977; further discussed <strong>and</strong><br />

reanalyzed by Shweder <strong>and</strong> D’Andrade, 1979). Shweder reanalyzed the data from<br />

a much earlier study of introversion <strong>and</strong> extroversion in children in a summer camp<br />

(Newcomb, 1929). The counselors at the camp provided daily ratings of the behavior<br />

of each child, on such items as “speaks with confidence of his own abilities,” “takes<br />

the initiative in organizing games,” <strong>and</strong> “spends more than an hour a day alone.”<br />

Because the daily ratings were made as soon as possible after the occurrence of the<br />

relevant behavior, the ratings were presumably as accurate as possible. From these<br />

data, it was possible to measure the correlation of every type of behavior with every<br />

other type of behavior over the entire summer. For example, the correlation between<br />

“gives loud <strong>and</strong> spontaneous expressions of delight or disapproval” <strong>and</strong> “talks more<br />

than his share of the time at the table” was .08, indicating a very weak relationship<br />

between these two behaviors; children who tend to exhibit the first behavior are not<br />

much more likely to exhibit the second behavior than children who do not exhibit the<br />

first. Shweder made a list of all of these correlations, one correlation for each pair<br />

of behaviors. (The correlations concerned children. All judgments made of a given<br />

child were averaged before the correlations were computed.)<br />

At the end of the summer, when the details of day-to-day happenings had been<br />

forgotten, the counselors filled out ratings of the same children on the same items.<br />

This situation is more like the questionnaires used in personality measurement, where<br />

ratings are based on long-term memory rather than specific day-to-day events. Once<br />

again, a list of correlations was made, one correlation for each pair of behaviors.<br />

Shweder then looked at the correlation of the two lists (of correlations). If the correlation<br />

of the two lists was high, those behavior pairs that had high correlations in<br />

one list would have high correlations in the other, <strong>and</strong> likewise for low correlations.<br />

For the two items described earlier, the correlation based on memory was .92 (rather<br />

than .08); thus, in this case, a high correlation in one list went with a low correlation<br />

in the other. Although the degree of difference observed for these two items was extreme,<br />

the two lists of correlations were, in general, not themselves correlated. The<br />

correlation between the daily-rating correlations <strong>and</strong> the memory-based correlations<br />

was only about .25. 2<br />

Why is there such a weak relationship between the correlations of daily ratings<br />

<strong>and</strong> the correlations of the memory ratings? Perhaps because of a sort of halo effect.<br />

People think that certain traits ought to go together, so when they give a high rating<br />

on one, they tend to give a high rating on the other as well, <strong>and</strong> vice versa.<br />

To get a better idea of what people think ought to go with what, in personality,<br />

Shweder asked a number of University of Chicago undergraduates to rate all of<br />

the pairs of behavioral descriptions from the summer-camp research for “conceptual<br />

similarity,” thereby creating a third list. These conceptual-similarity ratings corre-<br />

2 This was the average over several studies of this type (Shweder <strong>and</strong> D’Andrade, 1979, p. 1081).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!