02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

THE MEASUREMENT OF UTILITY 335<br />

The study involved a personal interview with each respondent. The interviewer<br />

used words <strong>and</strong> pictures to describe the damage done by the spill <strong>and</strong> how spills coud<br />

be prevented by using escort ships <strong>and</strong> other means. The most critical parts of the<br />

interview included the following:<br />

If the program was approved, here is how it would be paid for.<br />

All of the oil companies that take oil out of Alaska would pay a special<br />

one timetax .... Households like yours would also pay a special one<br />

time charge that would be added to their federal taxes ....<br />

Because everyone would bear part of the cost, we are using this survey<br />

to ask people how they would vote if they had the chance to vote on the<br />

program. . . .<br />

Of course, whether people would vote for or against the escort ship program<br />

depends on how much it will cost their household. At present,<br />

government officials estimate the program will cost your household a<br />

totalof$X....<br />

If the program cost your household a total of $X would you vote for the<br />

program or against it?<br />

The value of $X was either $10, $30, $60, or $120 for different respondents. Respondents<br />

who said yes were then asked about a higher amount (for example, $30,<br />

if the respondent would pay $10), <strong>and</strong> those who said no were asked about a lower<br />

amount. The method of asking for a yes-no vote does not actually tell how much<br />

each respondent is willing to pay. It can only tell whether the amount is more or<br />

less than each of the two values used. With many respondents, the researchers could<br />

estimate the median (middle) critical value, which ranged from $27 to $46 depending<br />

on how the estimate was made. In other CV studies, respondents are often asked<br />

outright how much they would be willing to pay, at most, for some program or good.<br />

The CV method is expensive. Even when respondents provide a number, the<br />

time required is large. Of course it is possible to modify the method <strong>and</strong> ask about<br />

several goods in one interview, but this is rarely done in practice. Researchers feel<br />

that they must specify each good with great care, <strong>and</strong> that takes as much time as most<br />

respondents are willing to spend. When, after half an hour, the respondent specifies<br />

only a vote, so that we do not even know the number she would provide, the method<br />

is more expensive still. Even more respondents are required in order to estimate the<br />

median. CV has still other problems, some of which are common to other methods<br />

too.<br />

Insensitivity to quantity<br />

For one thing, CV responses are insensitive to quantity. People are not willing to pay<br />

much more to prevent ten oil spills per year than to prevent one. Unless their price<br />

is very high, so that it dents significantly into their wealth, they should be willing to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!