02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

THE PSYCHOLOGY OF HYPOTHESIS TESTING 173<br />

5 could support the hypothesis, if the answer were no, since the hypothesis predicts<br />

a negative answer for this item. Certainly the subject expects her hypothesis to be<br />

supported, but this is true no matter what kind of sequence she gives.<br />

In sum, if subjects have a problem here, it is not in “trying to confirm.” They<br />

may have no problem at all. Subjects may well be trying to test alternative hypotheses<br />

(such as ascending even numbers less than 100). The hypotheses they have in<br />

mind may seem unlikely to us, but nothing in the experiment tells the subject what<br />

probabilities to assign to various alternative hypotheses.<br />

What subjects do in these studies is to provide tests that are congruent with their<br />

favored hypothesis. As noted, these studies have not shown that this violates any<br />

normative model. But perhaps it results from the use of a heuristic that might lead<br />

to such violations. We might call this a congruence heuristic: “To test a hypothesis,<br />

think of a result that would be found if the hypothesis were true <strong>and</strong> then look for that<br />

result (<strong>and</strong> do not worry about other hypotheses that might yield the same result).”<br />

Baron, Beattie, <strong>and</strong> Hershey (1988) found that subjects did indeed seem to use<br />

a “congruence heuristic” in which they favored tests or questions that gave a yes<br />

answer if their favored hypothesis was true, even if this question was nonnormative.<br />

Here is one of their test questions:<br />

A patient has a .8 probability of having Chamber-of-Commerce disease<br />

<strong>and</strong> a .2 probability of Elks disease. (He surely has one or the other.)<br />

A tetherscopic examination yields a positive result in 90% of patients<br />

with Chamber-of-Commerce disease <strong>and</strong> in 20% of patients without it<br />

(including those with some other disease). An intraocular smear yields a<br />

positive result in 90% of patients with Elks disease <strong>and</strong> in 10% of those<br />

without it. If you could do only one of these tests, which would it be?<br />

Why?<br />

The “tetherscopic” examination yields a yes answer if the favored hypothesis (“Chamber-of-Commerce<br />

disease”) is true. Many subjects chose the tetherscopic examination<br />

for this reason, even though the “intraocular smear” is a better test in this case.<br />

(To see why it is better, note that one could interpret a negative result on the intraocular<br />

smear as a positive indication of Chamber-of-Commerce disease; looking<br />

at it this way, the test has a 10% false-positive rate, instead of the 20% rate for the<br />

tetherscopic examination.)<br />

In another procedure, subjects were asked to imagine themselves in everyday situations,<br />

in which they could ask yes-no questions to test a hypothesis. They were<br />

asked to evaluate each question (with a numerical rating) as a test. They were later<br />

asked to give their own probability judgments for the hypothesis <strong>and</strong> for a yes answer<br />

to each question, first assuming the hypothesis to be true, <strong>and</strong> then assuming<br />

it to be false. In the following typical example, a subject gave the highest rating to<br />

the question that yielded a yes answer if the hypothesis was true, question 2, even<br />

though question 1 would be more informative, as determined from the subject’s own<br />

probabilities:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!