02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Practically all thinking involves beliefs in some way. We make decisions on the<br />

basis of beliefs about the outcomes of each option. If I want to drive to the city, I<br />

choose my route according to my beliefs about speed <strong>and</strong> safety. When we think<br />

about the strengths of our goals, such as the relative importance of speed <strong>and</strong> safety,<br />

we do this as well on the basis of beliefs, for example, beliefs about what it would<br />

feel like to have an automobile accident. Beliefs themselves can be the objects of<br />

thought. I can think about how much the construction on the expressway is likely to<br />

slow me down, given that it is Saturday afternoon. (Is there a sports event in town?)<br />

Part II explores three major approaches to the analysis of belief formation: probability<br />

theory, the theory of hypothesis testing, <strong>and</strong> the theory of correlation. It concludes<br />

with a chapter on biases in belief formation in general.<br />

Probability theory (Chapter 5) is a normative theory for evaluating numerically<br />

the strengths of our beliefs in various propositions. Like logic, it is a theory of<br />

inference, but unlike logic, it concerns degrees of belief rather than certainty. In<br />

Chapter 6, we shall see that our belief strengths, as manifest in our judgments of<br />

probability, violate this normative model seriously. It is not just that our numbers<br />

are not exactly correct. They are often systematically out of order. We believe that<br />

one event is more likely than another when the rest of our beliefs imply the opposite.<br />

We can take steps to guard against these violations. Actively open-minded thinking<br />

can help. When probability judgments are very important, computers can help us as<br />

well.<br />

The theory of hypothesis testing (Chapter 7) concerns the selection of evidence:<br />

asking the right question to find out how much to believe some hypothesis. If we<br />

ask questions well, we can make inferences from their answers that bring our beliefs<br />

into line with the available evidence as efficiently as possible. Scientists try to<br />

do this when they design experiments, <strong>and</strong> physicians try to do it when they order<br />

tests. Again, we shall find certain biases that cause us to depart from the normative<br />

model. Some of these biases can be ameliorated by actively open-minded thinking,<br />

especially by considering other hypotheses than those we favor.<br />

Chapter 8 deals with beliefs that concern the correlation between two variables.<br />

We tend to perceive data as showing correlations when we already believe that we<br />

will find a correlation. Chapter 9 shows that this distorting effect of prior belief<br />

occurs throughout our thinking, <strong>and</strong> the chapter also explores the causes of this bias<br />

<strong>and</strong> the conditions under which it occurs. The existence of this bias is the main<br />

justification for my claim that we need to remind ourselves to be open to new beliefs<br />

<strong>and</strong> to evidence against our old ones: Our natural tendency is often the opposite.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!