02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

394 MORAL JUDGMENT AND CHOICE<br />

appeal to the law against theft, or to the expectations of what a good husb<strong>and</strong> would<br />

do. At the highest level, which is not reached by many adults, people distinguish<br />

morality <strong>and</strong> convention <strong>and</strong> thus admit the possibility that the morally right thing to<br />

do might go against the law or social expectation.<br />

Kohlberg used open-ended interviews about moral dilemmas. Subsequent researchers<br />

have found that very young children can often distinguish morality <strong>and</strong><br />

convention if they are asked direct, simple questions. Some of the same researchers<br />

have defined more carefully the properties of different kinds of judgments.<br />

Specifically, Turiel (1983) <strong>and</strong> others following in his tradition (e.g., Nucci,<br />

2001) found that most children <strong>and</strong> adults in the U.S. <strong>and</strong> several other countries<br />

make a distinction between moral rules <strong>and</strong> conventional rules. Turiel <strong>and</strong> his collaborators<br />

asked the children whether it would be okay to call one’s teacher by her<br />

first name if everyone thought it was okay, or to bow instead of shaking h<strong>and</strong>s, or<br />

whether it would be okay for a bully to push another kid off the top of a slide if everyone<br />

thought it was okay. Even most second graders said it would be okay to change<br />

the conventions but not the moral rules: It would still be wrong to push someone off<br />

of a slide, even if everyone thought it was not wrong.<br />

Many children, however, fail to distinguish morality <strong>and</strong> convention. They think<br />

that it would be wrong to call the teacher by her first name, for a boy to come to class<br />

wearing a dress, or for children to take off their clothes at school when the weather<br />

got too hot, regardless of whether others thought it was wrong or not. Shweder,<br />

Mahapatra, <strong>and</strong> Miller (1988) found such responses among adults <strong>and</strong> children in a<br />

Hindu village in India.<br />

Haidt, Koller, <strong>and</strong> Dias (1993) presented subjects — children <strong>and</strong> adults in the<br />

U.S. <strong>and</strong> Brazil — with actions, some of which were intended to be both harmless<br />

<strong>and</strong> offensive, such as “A family’s dog was killed by a car in fron of their house.<br />

They had heard that dog meat was delicious, so they cut up the dog’s body <strong>and</strong> ate it.”<br />

Another case was, “A brother <strong>and</strong> sister like to kiss each other on the mouth. When<br />

nobody is around, they find a secret hiding place <strong>and</strong> kiss each other on the mouth,<br />

passionately.” Of interest was whether subjects considered the action to be morally<br />

wrong. This relied on two probe questions. One asked whether it was legitimate<br />

for others to stop or punish the activity. The other, which distinguished morality<br />

<strong>and</strong> convention, asked whether the activity was wrong even if everyone in a country<br />

thought it was acceptable. The subjects were also asked if they would be bothered<br />

witnessing the action, <strong>and</strong> whether the action had bad consequences. In general,<br />

subjects from higher social classes in both countries were less likely to see the actions<br />

as morally wrong. Some subjects in these groups, however, did see the actions as<br />

wrong, <strong>and</strong>, when they did this, they tended to say that there were bad consequences.<br />

The subjects in lower classes tended to see the actions as morally wrong. Although<br />

they did not think that the wrong actions were always harmful, they did usually<br />

say that they would be “bothered.” These children apparently failed to distinguish<br />

morality <strong>and</strong> convention in much the way that Kohlberg proposed.<br />

Other questions concern whether a rule can be changed by anyone (by citizens,<br />

by the pope, by ministers of a church) or whether it is still a moral requirement even

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!