02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

190 JUDGMENT OF CORRELATION AND CONTINGENCY<br />

Table 8.3: Incidence of response <strong>and</strong> outcome for 120 trials<br />

Outcome<br />

Response Score No score<br />

A 48 12<br />

B 48 12<br />

some control over the outcome even when they have no control at all has come to be<br />

called the illusion of control.<br />

These results were obtained despite the fact that subjects were explicitly instructed<br />

that “control means the ability to produce the No Score light as well as<br />

to produce the Score light.” Subjects were also explicitly warned that there might<br />

be no relation at all between their responses <strong>and</strong> the outcomes. The same results<br />

were obtained in the judgments of subjects who merely watched other subjects do<br />

the experiment. Therefore, we cannot explain the results in terms of some effect of<br />

the choice itself; the “spectator” subjects made no choices. The results were also<br />

obtained when subjects were explicitly asked to try to “control” the outcome rather<br />

than to “score”; these “control” subjects had to say aloud, in advance, which light<br />

they were trying to produce.<br />

In a questionnaire given after the experiment, subjects were shown tables of outcomes<br />

similar to Table 8.3. The figures in this table reveal that the Score light came<br />

on with a probability of .8 regardless of the response. Once again, even with the<br />

tables before them the subjects judged their degree of control in terms of the probability<br />

of success rather than in terms of the difference in results produced by the two<br />

buttons.<br />

Part of the subjects’ difficulty seemed to result from the fact that they had to press<br />

one button on each trial. When they were asked how much “control” they had over<br />

the outcome, they tended to assume that they had a fair amount, because, they reasoned,<br />

if they had not pushed any button, there would have been no outcome. Allan<br />

<strong>and</strong> Jenkins (1980) tried the procedure with a single button. The subjects could press<br />

or not press on each trial, <strong>and</strong> after 100 trials the subjects were asked to estimate the<br />

degree of influence they had, or the degree of connection between pressing the button<br />

<strong>and</strong> the outcome. In this procedure, subjects still showed the illusion of control.<br />

They still said they had some control (influence or connection) over the outcome,<br />

even when they actually had none. The illusion was smaller with one button than<br />

with two, however. Subjects tended to give lower ratings of their degree of control.<br />

In this study, subjects’ ratings were affected by the actual amount of control; subjects<br />

estimated that they had more control in a .9 − .1 condition (outcome appears with<br />

probability .9 when they press, .1 when they do not press) than in a .9 − .5 condition.<br />

With either one button or two, however, they thought they had more control in a

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!