02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

378 QUANTITATIVE JUDGMENT<br />

Kahneman <strong>and</strong> Tversky’s subjects), the worse the predictor is. In the extreme, if the<br />

subject were in the ninetieth percentile on shoe size, that would probably have no<br />

relation at all to grades, <strong>and</strong> your best guess would be that the student would have a<br />

GPA at the class mean of 2.5. Sense of humor is probably as useless as shoe size in its<br />

power to predict college GPA, so your best guess might be a little over 50% (or a little<br />

under, if you think that humor gets in the way of good grades). Mental concentration<br />

is probably a better predictor than sense of humor but not as good a predictor as the<br />

percentile rank in GPA itself, so the best prediction should be somewhere between<br />

2.5 <strong>and</strong> 3.5.<br />

To see why we should regress toward the mean, let us suppose a student scores<br />

at the ninetieth percentile on the mental concentration test, <strong>and</strong> let us ask whether<br />

we would expect the student to obtain a GPA of 3.5 (which, let us assume, is the<br />

ninetieth percentile for GPA), or higher, or lower. Mental concentration is not a perfect<br />

predictor of grades, so some of the students who score in the ninetieth percentile<br />

on the mental concentration test will do better than a GPA of 3.5, <strong>and</strong> some will do<br />

worse. More students will do worse than will do better, however, because there are<br />

more students with a GPA below 3.5 than there are with a GPA above it. There are,<br />

for example, more students with a GPA of 3.4 than with a GPA of 3.6, because 3.4 is<br />

closer to the mean GPA. It is therefore more likely that a r<strong>and</strong>omly selected student<br />

will have a GPA of 3.4 <strong>and</strong> a mental concentration score at the ninetieth percentile<br />

than a GPA of 3.6 <strong>and</strong> a mental concentration score at the ninetieth percentile. The<br />

same argument applies to GPAs of 3.3 <strong>and</strong> 3.7, 3.2 <strong>and</strong> 3.8, <strong>and</strong> so on. If we predict<br />

that students in the ninetieth percentile in mental concentration will have a GPA of<br />

3.5, we are, in effect, ignoring the prior probability of their GPAs being above or<br />

below 3.5 (as discussed in Chapter 6). We can think of the mental concentration<br />

score as a datum, from which we must infer the probability of various hypotheses<br />

about the student’s GPA. The hypotheses below 3.5 are more likely at the outset, so<br />

they remain more likely after the datum is obtained. Nonregressiveness may be<br />

overcome by thinking about missing data. Ganzach <strong>and</strong> Krantz (1991) gave subjects<br />

experience at predicting the college grade-point average (COL) from descriptions<br />

that included ACH <strong>and</strong> SAT (as described earlier in this chapter). When subjects<br />

then made predictions on the basis of single predictor (SAT or ACH), they regressed<br />

these predictions toward the mean, unlike other subjects who did not have the experience<br />

of using more than one predictor. It seems that these experienced subjects<br />

thought about the missing predictors <strong>and</strong> assumed moderate values for them. This<br />

heuristic will not always work, however, because it is not always possible to know<br />

what are the missing predictors.<br />

Another example of nonregressiveness, failure to regress enough toward the mean,<br />

is prediction of completion times (Buehler, Griffin, <strong>and</strong> Ross, 1994). Projects of all<br />

sorts, from massive works of construction to students’ papers to professors’ textbooks,<br />

are rarely completed in the amount of time originally estimated. Cost overruns<br />

are much more common than underruns (to the point where my spelling checker<br />

thinks that only the former is an English word). In 1957, for example, the Sydney<br />

(Australia) Opera House was predicted to cost $17 million <strong>and</strong> to be completed in

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!