02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

DETERMINANTS AND RELATED PHENOMENA 217<br />

believe that they had written the essay for a good reason. Because they did not do it<br />

for the money, they apparently convinced themselves that they had done it because<br />

they really agreed with the position more than they initially thought.<br />

In another classic study, Festinger <strong>and</strong> Carlsmith (1959) induced subjects to participate<br />

in a psychology experiment that they deliberately made boring <strong>and</strong> tedious.<br />

After the experiment, each subject was asked to convince the next subject (actually<br />

a confederate) that the experiment was interesting <strong>and</strong> fun. Half of the subjects were<br />

paid $1 for their participation in the experiment; the other half were paid $20. After<br />

this, the subjects were interviewed about their true opinion of the experiment. The<br />

group paid $1 had a more favorable opinion of the experiment.<br />

Festinger (1962) explains these results as a process of “reduction in cognitive<br />

dissonance.” When the choice is difficult, the reasons for one decision are “dissonant”<br />

with the reasons for the other, <strong>and</strong> the dissonance can be reduced by playing<br />

down the reasons for the choice not made or inventing reasons for the choice made<br />

(for example, that the dull experiment was really interesting).<br />

Surely we try to eliminate conflict among our beliefs. Most of our attempts to do<br />

this, however, are completely rational. When we find evidence against a belief that<br />

we favor, for example, we often reduce the strength of the belief, so as to “reduce the<br />

dissonance.” These experiments, however, seem to show some sort of irrationality.<br />

What is irrational here? The idea of “dissonance reduction” does not by itself seem<br />

to capture it.<br />

The reason for this sort of postdecisional change could be that people like to<br />

believe that they are “good” decision makers — both morally good <strong>and</strong> intelligent.<br />

They change their beliefs about their reasons for having made a decision so that their<br />

beliefs fit their desire that they be good in these ways. When they write an essay<br />

opposing their real view for only a small amount of pay, it is easiest to justify that<br />

decision (a bad financial deal) by thinking that they truly have some sympathy for<br />

that position. Likewise, they may justify doing a boring task for a small amount of<br />

pay by thinking that they actually liked the task. Similarly, when they make a difficult<br />

decision between two choices, they may later have doubts about whether they made<br />

the best decision unless, in retrospect, they see the decision as not so difficult at all;<br />

they therefore play down the value of the rejected choice. These experiments seem<br />

to illustrate a form of wishful thinking, where the “wish” is the desire to have been<br />

a good decision maker, <strong>and</strong> the beliefs that are affected are those about reasons for<br />

having made a decision.<br />

A number of experiments support this view. Cooper (1971), for example, found<br />

that the effect of past decisions on beliefs was larger when the outcome of the decision<br />

could be foreseen than when it could not. The decision in question involved<br />

agreeing to work with another subject (a confederate) in a task in which the amount<br />

of payment depended on the performance of both subjects. In order to receive high<br />

payment, both subjects had to solve aptitude test problems <strong>and</strong>, after doing each<br />

problem, indicate accurately whether they had answered it correctly or not. The subject<br />

was told that her partner was either “too timid to publicly state that she had [the<br />

problem] correct” or else was “a little too sure of herself.” In fact, the partner did

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!