02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

THE PSYCHOLOGY OF HYPOTHESIS TESTING 181<br />

Table 7.4: Joint probabilities, p(H & D) =p(D|H) · p(H), for water example<br />

Question Answer H ∼H<br />

1. jug in refrigerator yes .085 .12<br />

no .765 .03<br />

2. refrigerator old yes .595 .06<br />

no .255 .09<br />

hypothesis is true. The hypothesis H is that the refrigerator stopped working <strong>and</strong> the<br />

ice melted. Consider two possible data, D1, a yes answer to “Had there been a large<br />

jug of water in the refrigerator?”, <strong>and</strong> D2, a yes answer to “Was the refrigerator<br />

old?” Assume that p(H) =.85 (so that p(H2) = .15), that p(D1|H) =.1, p(D1|∼<br />

H) =.8, p(D2|H) =.7, <strong>and</strong> p(D2|∼H) =.4. Using the multiplication rule to<br />

multiply each conditional probability by the prior probability of the corresponding<br />

hypothesis, we get the results in Table 7.4. (Bear in mind that “no” is the opposite<br />

of “yes,” so the sum of these two joint probabilities must equal the probability of the<br />

hypothesis.) Without asking either question, the probability of guessing correctly<br />

is .85, the probability of H. If we ask question 1, we could guess correctly with<br />

probability .885 (.765 + .12) after asking the question, but question 2 provides no<br />

benefit at all, since we would guess that H is true no matter what the answer to the<br />

question.<br />

Question 1 will this have a greater beneficial effect on the probability of guessing<br />

correctly, <strong>and</strong> we can determine this — given our probabilities — before we even<br />

ask the question. We can list the possible answers to a given question or the possible<br />

results of a test, <strong>and</strong> we can ask how likely each result is, given each hypothesis, <strong>and</strong><br />

how much it would improve our chance of guessing correctly.<br />

Utility <strong>and</strong> alternative hypotheses<br />

We can use this approach to underst<strong>and</strong> the value of considering alternative hypotheses,<br />

as discussed earlier in this chapter. Suppose that we think that the probability<br />

that a patient has disease A is .8. We know of a test that yields a positive result in<br />

80% of the patients with disease A. Is the test worth doing?<br />

Many would say yes. I suggest that we would need to know more before we<br />

decide. A good heuristic is to ask what other disease the patient may have if she does<br />

not have disease A. Suppose the answer is disease B. Then we would want to know<br />

what the probability of a positive result from that same test would be if the patient<br />

had disease B. Suppose the probability is .4. Then, for 100 imaginary patients, we<br />

would have a table like Table 7.5. Even if the test is negative, disease A will be our<br />

best guess. Therefore, the test has 0 utility. The problem is that the test has too high<br />

a “false-positive” rate (8 out of 20 patients, or .4). Perhaps we could find a more

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!