02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

HEURISTICS AND BIASES IN PROBABILITY 147<br />

Tom W.<br />

In perhaps the clearest example of the representativeness heuristic, Kahneman <strong>and</strong><br />

Tversky (1973) asked one group of subjects to estimate the proportion of first-year<br />

graduate students in the United States in nine different fields (business administration,<br />

computer science, engineering, humanities <strong>and</strong> education, law, library science,<br />

medicine, physical <strong>and</strong> life sciences, <strong>and</strong> social science <strong>and</strong> social work). Subjects<br />

judged computer science to be the second smallest (7% of all graduate students) of<br />

the nine fields, with social science <strong>and</strong> social work the second largest (17%). (These<br />

judgments were realistic at the time the study was done, around 1970.) These proportions<br />

constitute the prior probabilities, which are often called the base rates, before<br />

any additional evidence about a particular graduate student is provided.<br />

Another group of subjects was given the following personality sketch of a firstyear<br />

graduate student (1973, p. 238):<br />

Tom W. is of high intelligence, although lacking in true creativity. He<br />

has a need for order <strong>and</strong> clarity, <strong>and</strong> for neat <strong>and</strong> tidy systems in which<br />

every detail finds its appropriate place. His writing is rather dull <strong>and</strong> mechanical,<br />

occasionally enlivened by somewhat corny puns <strong>and</strong> by flashes<br />

of imagination of the sci-fi type. He has a strong drive for competence.<br />

He seems to have little feel <strong>and</strong> little sympathy for other people <strong>and</strong><br />

does not enjoy interacting with others. Self-centered, he nonetheless<br />

has a deep moral sense.<br />

The subjects were asked to judge how similar Tom W. was to the typical graduate<br />

student in each of the nine fields. The subjects considered Tom to be most similar<br />

to computer-science students <strong>and</strong> least similar to social-science <strong>and</strong> social work students.<br />

These similarity ratings correspond to the representativeness of the sketch of<br />

each of the categories.<br />

A third group was told that the sketch of Tom had been written by a psychologist,<br />

on the basis of projective tests, when Tom was a senior in high school. This group of<br />

subjects was asked to rank the nine different fields according to the probabilities of<br />

Tom’s being in them. The rankings matched almost perfectly the rankings given by<br />

the similarity group. Tom was considered to be most likely to be studying computer<br />

science <strong>and</strong> least likely to be studying social science or social work. The probability<br />

ratings were not related at all to the prior probabilities. When subjects are asked to<br />

make a probability judgment, they apparently base it entirely on their beliefs about<br />

similarity, or representativeness, <strong>and</strong> not at all on their beliefs about prior probability.<br />

(It can be assumed that this group of subjects would have given the same prior probability<br />

<strong>and</strong> similarity ratings as the other groups; all groups were drawn from the same<br />

population of respondents to an advertisement in a college newspaper.) This neglect<br />

of prior probability leads to systematic errors in ranking the fields in probability, that<br />

is, in saying which of two fields was more likely. The error is therefore not just a<br />

matter of not assigning the right numbers to beliefs.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!