02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

22 WHAT IS THINKING?<br />

We might be tempted to suppose that the valve theory is maintained by its functional<br />

value in saving fuel rather than by the ready availability of analogies with other<br />

valves (accelerators, faucets) <strong>and</strong> by its explanatory value. This conclusion does not<br />

follow. To draw it, we would need to argue that the functional value of the valve<br />

theory causes the theory to be maintained (Elster, 1979). Are people really sensitive<br />

to the amount of fuel they use? People’s beliefs sometimes are for the best, but, as<br />

McCloskey argues, sometimes they are not.<br />

This example is a particularly good illustration of naive theories, because it seems<br />

likely that the subjects have actually thought about how thermostats work. They<br />

have had to face the issue in learning how to use them. In the previous examples<br />

from college physics, it is not clear that the subjects really “had” any theories before<br />

they were confronted with the problems given them by the experimenters. They may<br />

simply have constructed answers to the problems on the spot. The fact that their<br />

answers often correspond to traditional theories simply reflects the fact (as it would<br />

on any account) that these theories explain the most obvious phenomena <strong>and</strong> are<br />

based on the most obvious analogies. After all, balls thrown with spin on them keep<br />

spinning; why should not balls shot out of a curved tube keep curving as well?<br />

The home heat-control theories seem to provide yet another example of restructuring<br />

(assuming that some people change from the valve theory to the feedback<br />

theory). Like the Copernican theory of astronomy, the fully correct theory requires<br />

new concepts, such as the concept of heat flow over a temperature difference <strong>and</strong> that<br />

of radiant heat. The theory establishes new relationships among concepts, such as<br />

thermostat settings <strong>and</strong> heat flow. It also explains different phenomena, such as the<br />

fact that the house temperature stays roughly at the setting on the thermostat.<br />

Underst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

Students <strong>and</strong> their teachers often make a distinction between underst<strong>and</strong>ing something<br />

<strong>and</strong> “just memorizing it” (or perhaps just not learning it at all). Everyone wants<br />

to learn with underst<strong>and</strong>ing <strong>and</strong> teach for underst<strong>and</strong>ing, but there is a lot of misunderst<strong>and</strong>ing<br />

about what underst<strong>and</strong>ing is. The issue has a history worth reviewing.<br />

Wertheimer <strong>and</strong> Katona<br />

Max Wertheimer (1945/1959), one of the founders of Gestalt psychology in the early<br />

part of the last century, is the psychologist who called our attention most forcefully<br />

to the problem of underst<strong>and</strong>ing. Wertheimer’s main example was the formula for<br />

finding the area of the parallelogram, A = b · h, where A is the area, b is the base,<br />

<strong>and</strong> h is the height. Wertheimer examined a group of students who had learned this<br />

formula to their teacher’s satisfaction. On close examination, though, they turned<br />

out not to underst<strong>and</strong> it. They could apply it in familiar cases such as the following<br />

parallelogram:

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!