02.03.2013 Views

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

Thinking and Deciding

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

92 LOGIC<br />

The same subject did not choose the 6 card for either rule. Again, for the negative<br />

rule, the subject gave a correct justification for not choosing the 6 card: “Any letter<br />

may be on the other side; therefore [there is] no way of knowing if the statement is<br />

true.” (If only the subject had applied this argument to the 3 card!) In the affirmative<br />

condition, however, the subject justified not choosing the 6 card as follows: “If numbers<br />

are fairly r<strong>and</strong>om, then there may be any letter on the other side, thereby giving<br />

no indication unless the letter is B.” Again, the subject could have chosen this card<br />

<strong>and</strong> justified it in the same way as the 3 card was justified for the negative rule.<br />

In sum, subjects’ justifications tend to be correct when they have chosen the correct<br />

cards, <strong>and</strong> incorrect when they have chosen the incorrect cards. Yet the justifications<br />

seem to play little role in determining the choice itself, for the choice remains<br />

much the same whether the rule is stated affirmatively or negatively. It would appear<br />

that the justification is indeed after the fact, that it is the result of a search for<br />

evidence in favor of a decision already made.<br />

Content effects<br />

Performance in the four-card task is affected by the content of the rules. (See Evans,<br />

1982, ch. 9, for a review.) The clearest effects concern bureaucratic rules. Johnson-<br />

Laird, Legrenzi, <strong>and</strong> Legrenzi (1972) showed adult subjects in Engl<strong>and</strong> a drawing<br />

of sealed <strong>and</strong> open stamped envelopes <strong>and</strong> asked them, “Which of the following<br />

envelopes must you examine to test the rule: If a letter is sealed, then it has a 5d<br />

stamp on it?” 6<br />

These subjects had an easy time figuring out that one had to check the closed envelope<br />

<strong>and</strong> the 4d envelope in order to test this rule. However, when Griggs <strong>and</strong> Cox<br />

(1982) attempted to replicate this experiment using college students in the United<br />

States, there was no difference between the envelope condition <strong>and</strong> a control condition<br />

with abstract materials (like the original four-card task described earlier). The<br />

American students were helped, however, when they were asked to imagine themselves<br />

as police officers whose job it was to test the rule “If a person is drinking beer,<br />

then the person must be over nineteen years of age.” In this case, most students understood<br />

that the people to check are those drinking beer (to find their age) or those<br />

nineteen or under (to see whether they are drinking beer).<br />

These results indicate that the mere concreteness of the envelope condition did<br />

not help to make the problem easier. Rather, thinking was helped either by familiarity<br />

6 A “5d” stamp is a stamp worth five pence.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!