05.03.2013 Views

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Language</strong> Socialization 441<br />

across contexts (Bronson & Watson-Gegeo, 2008; Garrett, 2008; Kulick &<br />

Schieffelin, 2004; Watson-Gegeo, 2004). One reason for this prolonged<br />

engagement with the community, which is typical of ethnography, is to<br />

underst<strong>and</strong> recurring cultural <strong>and</strong> linguistic patterns of interaction (processes<br />

of socialization). Another reason is to observe how learners’ abilities,<br />

behaviors <strong>and</strong> orientations to learning <strong>and</strong> participating in the target<br />

practices evolve over time either in the manner anticipated or expected or,<br />

rather, in unanticipated ways (i.e. revealing outcomes of socialization).<br />

There is generally an attempt to bring together macro <strong>and</strong> micro-analyses<br />

in language socialization research that takes into account the wider context<br />

<strong>and</strong> how the sociocultural context is also constituted in <strong>and</strong> by microlevel<br />

linguistic features.<br />

Even ethnographic language socialization research has, however, paid<br />

more attention to the interactional <strong>and</strong> linguistic processes of socialization<br />

in real interactional time than to the systematic study of outcomes: what<br />

people are able to do after signifi cant, cumulative exposure to the language,<br />

culture <strong>and</strong> relevant practices, <strong>and</strong> not only what they are expected<br />

to do <strong>and</strong> are currently able to do. Indeed, since language socialization is<br />

known to be a lifelong <strong>and</strong> lifewide process, no single study can really be<br />

suffi ciently longitudinal (or both broad <strong>and</strong> deep) to capture the ebb <strong>and</strong><br />

fl ow of socialization <strong>and</strong> its many milestones, twists <strong>and</strong> turns across the<br />

lifespan of an individual. In studies more typically limited in duration, the<br />

process of language education <strong>and</strong> enculturation seems to trump detailed<br />

analyses <strong>and</strong> evidence of linguistic, affective <strong>and</strong> other (e.g. cognitive)<br />

outcomes of language socialization in longitudinal perspective.<br />

One strategy often used in narrowing the scope of analysis in language<br />

socialization studies is to select a commonplace yet signifi cant activity<br />

(speech event) that participants routinely engage in, such as bedtime or<br />

mealtime prayers, dinner time conversations, oral academic presentations,<br />

guided recitations, or story-telling or story-reading routines, <strong>and</strong> then to<br />

document how they unfold over time <strong>and</strong> the relevant components, participation<br />

structures, <strong>and</strong> related cultural tools or artifacts. Other studies,<br />

however, focus less on the particular activity settings <strong>and</strong> more on the<br />

sociolinguistic dispositions <strong>and</strong> forms to be mastered, such as those<br />

entailed in showing respect to one’s elders (Howard, 2008) or politeness<br />

<strong>and</strong> empathy (Cook, 2008).<br />

As in much research in the ethnography of communication or comparable<br />

ethnographic research, participants’ own perspectives on the socialization<br />

process are generally solicited by researchers through interviews if<br />

interviewees have the necessary linguistic or metalinguistic skills <strong>and</strong><br />

maturity to refl ect on them or if interpreters are available otherwise.<br />

Exceptions or counter-examples <strong>and</strong> not only typical instances of socialization<br />

toward desired goals should be reported by researchers, a common<br />

recommendation for interpretive qualitative research (Duff, 2008c).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!