05.03.2013 Views

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

Sociolinguistics and Language Education.pdf

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

<strong>Language</strong> <strong>and</strong> Culture 459<br />

<strong>and</strong> linguistic resources that participants in interaction recruit, including<br />

ethnosemantic categories (Eglin, 1980), systems of person reference <strong>and</strong><br />

address (Braun, 1988; Eglin, 1980) or grammatical forms associated with<br />

particular registers <strong>and</strong> styles (Cook, 2008). But the analytical interest in<br />

practices directs the researcher’s glance away from inferred abstract systems<br />

of categories <strong>and</strong> linguistic forms <strong>and</strong> toward the ways that participants<br />

in social interaction engage linguistic <strong>and</strong> other semiotic resources<br />

to accomplish practical, situated actions (Bilmes, 2008; Enfi eld & Stivers,<br />

2007; Hester & Eglin, 1997).<br />

The scope, unity, <strong>and</strong> diversity of culture<br />

Some further conceptual clarifi cation concerning the scope <strong>and</strong> differentiation<br />

of culture is in order. In everyday use as well as in much academic<br />

discourse, culture is commonly understood as particular to a given<br />

social group <strong>and</strong> as shared within that group. Claims to external (intergroup)<br />

distinctiveness <strong>and</strong> internal (intragroup) homogeneity can serve as<br />

powerful ideological tools to implement political agendas, from 19th century<br />

nation building in Europe to current restrictions on immigration <strong>and</strong><br />

equitable social <strong>and</strong> political rights for residents of different backgrounds<br />

in many parts of the world. Yet, against popular belief <strong>and</strong> research traditions<br />

in some social sciences, formulations of culture as exclusively particular<br />

are diffi cult to sustain empirically. As Ochs notes,<br />

Culture is not only tied to the local <strong>and</strong> unique, it is also a property of<br />

our humanity <strong>and</strong> as such expected to assume some culturally universal<br />

characteristics across communities, codes <strong>and</strong> users. (. . .) there are<br />

certain commonalties across the world’s language communities <strong>and</strong><br />

communities of practice in the linguistic means to constitute certain<br />

situational meanings. (Ochs, 1996: 425)<br />

The range of language-mediated cultural phenomena by which social<br />

members anywhere conduct their lives is large, <strong>and</strong> we will have more to<br />

say about some of them in the following sections. They include languagemediated<br />

social acts (‘speech acts’) <strong>and</strong> activities, narration <strong>and</strong> reporting<br />

the speech of others. Social identities, relationships <strong>and</strong> epistemic <strong>and</strong><br />

affective stance are indexed through linguistic <strong>and</strong> other semiotic resources<br />

(e.g. Coupl<strong>and</strong>, 2007; Ochs, 1996). Participation in talk exchanges is made<br />

possible through generic interactional procedures, such as turn-taking,<br />

sequence organization <strong>and</strong> repair (Drew, 2005). These fundamental interactional<br />

organizations are always confi gured to the local circumstances<br />

<strong>and</strong> are therefore responsive to, as well as constitutive of, culture-specifi c<br />

social norms <strong>and</strong> priorities, but such infl ections of local culture do not run<br />

counter to the overwhelming evidence that the procedural infrastructure<br />

of interaction is culturally shared (Moerman, 1988; Schegloff, 2006).

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!