27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

Interview<strong>in</strong>g: Unstructured and Semistructured 217<br />

nean. I asked him how he knew this, and he challenged me to document it.<br />

Savas had decided that there was someth<strong>in</strong>g important that I needed to know<br />

and he maneuvered the <strong>in</strong>terview around to make sure I learned it.<br />

This led to about 3 hours of pa<strong>in</strong>stak<strong>in</strong>g work. We counted the number of<br />

seasons he’d been to sea over a 46-year career (he remembered that he hadn’t<br />

worked at all dur<strong>in</strong>g 1943 because of ‘‘someth<strong>in</strong>g to do with the war’’). We<br />

figured conservatively the number of days he’d spent at sea, the average number<br />

of dives per trip, and the average depth and time per dive. We joked about<br />

the tendency of divers to exaggerate their exploits and about how fragile<br />

human memory is when it comes to this k<strong>in</strong>d of detail.<br />

It was difficult to stay on the subject, because Savas was such a good raconteur<br />

and a perceptive analyst of Kalymnian life. The <strong>in</strong>terview meandered off<br />

on <strong>in</strong>terest<strong>in</strong>g tangents, but after a while, either Savas or I would steer it back<br />

to the issue at hand. In the end, discount<strong>in</strong>g heavily for both exaggeration and<br />

faulty recall, we reckoned that he’d spent at least 10,000 hours—about a year<br />

and a fourth, count<strong>in</strong>g each day as a full 24 hours—under water and had<br />

walked the distance between Alexandria and Tunis at least three times.<br />

The exact numbers really didn’t matter. What did matter was that Savas<br />

Ergas had a really good sense of what he thought I needed to know about the<br />

life of a sponge diver. It was I, the <strong>in</strong>terviewer, who def<strong>in</strong>ed the focus of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>terview; but it was Savas, the respondent, who determ<strong>in</strong>ed the content. And<br />

was I ever glad he did.<br />

Prob<strong>in</strong>g<br />

The key to successful <strong>in</strong>terview<strong>in</strong>g is learn<strong>in</strong>g how to probe effectively—<br />

that is, to stimulate a respondent to produce more <strong>in</strong>formation, without <strong>in</strong>ject<strong>in</strong>g<br />

yourself so much <strong>in</strong>to the <strong>in</strong>teraction that you only get a reflection of yourself<br />

<strong>in</strong> the data. Suppose you ask, ‘‘Have you ever been away from the village<br />

to work?’’ and the <strong>in</strong>formant says, ‘‘Yes.’’ The next question (the probe) is:<br />

‘‘Like where?’’ Suppose the answer is, ‘‘Oh, several different places.’’ The<br />

correct response is not, ‘‘Pachuca? Querétaro? Mexico City?’’ but, ‘‘Like<br />

where? Could you name some of the places where you’ve gone to get work?’’<br />

There are many k<strong>in</strong>ds of probes that you can use <strong>in</strong> an <strong>in</strong>terview. (In what<br />

follows, I draw on the important work by Kluckhohn [1945], Merton et al.<br />

[1956], Kahn and Cannell [1957], Whyte [1960, 1984], Dohrenwend and<br />

Richardson [1965], Gorden [1975], Hyman and Cobb [1975], Warwick and<br />

L<strong>in</strong><strong>in</strong>ger [1975], Reed and Stimson [1985], and on my own experience and<br />

that of my students.)

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!