27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

444 Chapter 15<br />

Ixmiquilpan (Mexico) to cover 10 meters of sidewalk at various times of the<br />

day. I have stood <strong>in</strong> crowded outdoor bazaars <strong>in</strong> Mexico, watch<strong>in</strong>g and record<strong>in</strong>g<br />

differences between Indians and non-Indians <strong>in</strong> the amount and k<strong>in</strong>ds of<br />

produce purchased.<br />

I have never felt the slightest ethical qualm about hav<strong>in</strong>g made these observations.<br />

In my op<strong>in</strong>ion, passive deception is ethically aseptic. Ultimately,<br />

however, the responsibility for the choice of method, and for the practical,<br />

human consequences of us<strong>in</strong>g a particular method, rests with you, the <strong>in</strong>dividual<br />

researcher. You can’t foist off that responsibility on ‘‘the profession,’’ or<br />

on some ‘‘code of ethics.’’<br />

Are you disturbed by the fact that Humphreys did his research at all, or only<br />

by the fact that he came close to compromis<strong>in</strong>g his <strong>in</strong>formants? As you answer<br />

that question for yourself, you’ll have a better idea of where you stand on the<br />

issue of disguised field observation.<br />

Behavior Trace Studies<br />

Th<strong>in</strong>k of trace studies as behavioral archeology. Do people <strong>in</strong> different cultures<br />

really have a different sense of time? Lev<strong>in</strong>e and Bartlett (1984) went to<br />

12 cities <strong>in</strong> six countries and noted the time on 15 randomly chosen bank<br />

clocks <strong>in</strong> each city. Then they measured the difference between the time<br />

shown on the clocks and the time reported by the local telephone company <strong>in</strong><br />

each city. The most accurate public clocks were Japanese—off by an average<br />

of just 34 seconds. U.S. clocks were next (off by an average of 54 seconds),<br />

followed by the clocks <strong>in</strong> Taiwan, England, and Italy (71 seconds, 72 seconds,<br />

and 90 seconds, respectively). Indonesia came <strong>in</strong> last, at 189 seconds.<br />

Here you have hard, archeological evidence of clock-sett<strong>in</strong>g behavior across<br />

six countries. Real people had set those 15 clocks <strong>in</strong> each city, and real people<br />

were responsible for mak<strong>in</strong>g sure that the clocks were adjusted from time to<br />

time. Lev<strong>in</strong>e and Bartlett looked at whether differences <strong>in</strong> the average deviation<br />

of the clocks from the real time predicted differences <strong>in</strong> the rate of heart<br />

disease.<br />

They don’t. The country with the lowest rate of heart disease, Japan, has<br />

the most accurate clocks and the fastest overall pace of life (as measured by<br />

several other <strong>in</strong>dicators). Apparently, accord<strong>in</strong>g to Lev<strong>in</strong>e and Bartlett, it’s<br />

possible <strong>in</strong> some cultures to be hard work<strong>in</strong>g without be<strong>in</strong>g hard driv<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

Sechrest and Flores (1969) recorded and analyzed bathroom graffiti <strong>in</strong> a<br />

sample of men’s public toilets <strong>in</strong> Manilla and Chicago. They wanted to exam<strong>in</strong>e<br />

attitudes toward sexuality <strong>in</strong> the two cultures. The results were strik<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

There was no difference <strong>in</strong> the percentage of graffiti <strong>in</strong> the two cities that dealt

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!