27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

324 Chapter 12<br />

those three cases would be responsible for six errors—you’d have to stick <strong>in</strong><br />

two pluses for each of the cases to make them come out accord<strong>in</strong>g to the<br />

hypothesis. Yes, you could make it just three, not six errors, by stick<strong>in</strong>g a<br />

m<strong>in</strong>us sign <strong>in</strong> column 3. Some researchers use this scor<strong>in</strong>g method, but I prefer<br />

the more conservative method of scor<strong>in</strong>g more errors. It keeps you on your<br />

toes.<br />

F<strong>in</strong>ally, we don’t expect that m<strong>in</strong>us sign <strong>in</strong> column 2 of respondent 16’s<br />

data. That case creates just one error (you only need to put <strong>in</strong> one plus to make<br />

it come out right). All together, that makes 3 6 1 10 errors <strong>in</strong> the<br />

attempt to reproduce a perfect scale. For table 12.1, the CR is<br />

1 (10/48) .79<br />

which is to say that the data come with<strong>in</strong> 21% of scal<strong>in</strong>g perfectly. By convention,<br />

a coefficient of reproducibility of .90 or greater is accepted as a significant<br />

approximation of a perfect scale (Guttman 1950). I’m will<strong>in</strong>g to settle for<br />

around .85, especially with the conservative method for scor<strong>in</strong>g errors, but .79<br />

just isn’t up to it, so these data fail the Guttman test for unidimensionality.<br />

Some Examples of a Guttman Scale<br />

Robert Carneiro (1962, 1970) had an idea that cultural evolution is orderly<br />

and cumulative. If he is right, then cultures evolve by add<strong>in</strong>g certa<strong>in</strong> traits <strong>in</strong><br />

an orderly way and should show a Guttman-scale-like pattern. Carneiro coded<br />

100 cultures for 354 traits and looked at the pattern. Table 12.2 shows a sample<br />

of 12 societies and 11 traits.<br />

When you collect data on cases, you don’t know what (if any) pattern will<br />

emerge, so you pretty much grab cases and code them for traits <strong>in</strong> random<br />

order. The 12 societies and traits <strong>in</strong> Table 12.2a are <strong>in</strong> random order.<br />

The first th<strong>in</strong>g to do is arrange the pluses and m<strong>in</strong>uses <strong>in</strong> their ‘‘best’’ possible<br />

order—the order that conforms most to the perfect Guttman scale—and<br />

compute the CR. We look for the trait that occurs most frequently (the one<br />

with the most pluses across the row) and place that one at the bottom of the<br />

matrix. The most frequently occurr<strong>in</strong>g trait is the existence of special religious<br />

practitioners. Then we look for the next most frequent trait and put it on the<br />

next to the bottom row of the matrix.<br />

We keep do<strong>in</strong>g this until we rearrange the data to take advantage of whatever<br />

underly<strong>in</strong>g pattern is hid<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> the matrix. The best arrangement of the<br />

pluses and m<strong>in</strong>uses is shown <strong>in</strong> table 12.2b. Now we can count up the ‘‘errors’’<br />

<strong>in</strong> the matrix and compute Guttman’s coefficient of reproducibility. For these<br />

12 societies and 11 traits, the coefficient is a perfect 1.0.<br />

Of course, it’s one th<strong>in</strong>g to f<strong>in</strong>d this k<strong>in</strong>d of blatant pattern <strong>in</strong> a matrix of

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!