27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

694 Chapter 21<br />

Gans and Wood (1985) used DFA technique for classify<strong>in</strong>g Samoan women<br />

as ‘‘traditional’’ or ‘‘modern’’ with respect to their ideal family size. If women<br />

stated that they wanted three or fewer children, Gans and Wood placed them<br />

<strong>in</strong> a category they labeled ‘‘modern.’’ Women who said they wanted four or<br />

more children were labeled ‘‘traditional.’’ DFA showed that just six of the<br />

many variables that Gans and Wood had collected allowed them to classify<br />

correctly which category a woman belonged to <strong>in</strong> 75% of all cases. The variables<br />

were such th<strong>in</strong>gs as age, own<strong>in</strong>g a car, level of education, etc.<br />

It would have been ridiculous for Gans and Wood to have asked women<br />

straight out: ‘‘Are you traditional or modern when it comes to the number of<br />

children you’d like?’’ DFA (comb<strong>in</strong>ed with on-the-ground ethnography) gave<br />

them a good picture of the variables that go <strong>in</strong>to Samoan women’s desired<br />

family size.<br />

Similarly, Comitas and I were able to describe the attitud<strong>in</strong>al components<br />

of gender role changes by us<strong>in</strong>g DFA, and our prediction rate of 70% was<br />

significantly better than the 50% we’d have gotten by chance, given our sampl<strong>in</strong>g<br />

design. If you’re careful about how you <strong>in</strong>terpret the results of a discrim<strong>in</strong>ant<br />

function analysis, it can be a really important addition to your statistical<br />

tool kit.<br />

And F<strong>in</strong>ally . . .<br />

In a world of thousands of variables and millions of comb<strong>in</strong>ations of variables,<br />

how do you decide what to test? There is no magic formula. My advice<br />

is to follow every hunch you get. Some researchers <strong>in</strong>sist that you have a good<br />

theoretical reason for <strong>in</strong>clud<strong>in</strong>g variables <strong>in</strong> your design and that you have a<br />

theory-driven reason to test for relations among variables once you have data.<br />

They po<strong>in</strong>t out that anyone can make up an explanation for any relation or<br />

lack of relation after see<strong>in</strong>g a table of data or a correlation coefficient.<br />

This is very good advice, but I th<strong>in</strong>k it’s a bit too restrictive, for three reasons:<br />

1. I th<strong>in</strong>k that data analysis should be lots of fun, and it can’t be unless it’s based<br />

on follow<strong>in</strong>g your hunches. Most relations are easy to expla<strong>in</strong>, and peculiar relations<br />

beg for theories to expla<strong>in</strong> them. You just have to be very careful not to<br />

conjure up support for every statistically significant relation, merely because it<br />

happens to turn up. There is a delicate balance between be<strong>in</strong>g clever enough to<br />

expla<strong>in</strong> an unexpected f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>g and just pla<strong>in</strong> reach<strong>in</strong>g too far. As usual, there is<br />

no substitute for th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g hard about your data.<br />

2. It is really up to you dur<strong>in</strong>g research design to be as clever as you can <strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>k<strong>in</strong>g<br />

up variables to test. You’re entitled to <strong>in</strong>clude some variables <strong>in</strong> your research

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!