27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

672 Chapter 21<br />

Path analysis lets you test a particular theory about the relations among a<br />

system of variables, but it doesn’t produce the theory; that’s your job. In the<br />

case of Niwan Witz, for example, Thomas specified that he wanted his path<br />

analysis to test a particular model <strong>in</strong> which wealth causes leadership. The<br />

results were strong, lead<strong>in</strong>g Thomas to reject the null hypothesis that there<br />

really is no causal relation between wealth and leadership. But even the strong<br />

results that Thomas got don’t prove anyth<strong>in</strong>g. In fact, Thomas noted that an<br />

alternative theory is plausible. It might be that leadership <strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong>dividuals (wherever<br />

they get it from) causes them to get wealthy rather than the other way<br />

around.<br />

Path analysis often serves as reality therapy for social scientists. It’s fun to<br />

build conceptual models—to th<strong>in</strong>k through a problem and hypothesize how<br />

variables are l<strong>in</strong>ked to each other—but our models are often much more complicated<br />

than they need to be. Path analysis can help us out of this fix. We can<br />

test several plausible theories and see which is most powerful. But <strong>in</strong> the end,<br />

you’re left out there, all by yourself, defend<strong>in</strong>g your theory on the basis of<br />

whatever data are available right now.<br />

Multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity<br />

Multivariate models are subject to a problem that simply can’t exist when<br />

you have one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable: Independent variables can be correlated.<br />

In fact, when two variables both strongly predict a third, you’d expect the first<br />

two to be correlated. This multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity means that you may not be able<br />

to tell the <strong>in</strong>fluence of one <strong>in</strong>dependent variable free from the <strong>in</strong>fluence of the<br />

<strong>in</strong>dependent variables with which it is correlated.<br />

One way to avoid this problem is to conduct true experiments and noth<strong>in</strong>g<br />

but true experiments. By assign<strong>in</strong>g research participants randomly to control<br />

and experimental groups, we ensure that any correlation between <strong>in</strong>dependent<br />

variables is the result of chance. Nice work if you can get it, but most of what<br />

we want to study <strong>in</strong> anthropology requires fieldwork or survey research where<br />

th<strong>in</strong>gs get messy.<br />

Fortunately, multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity distorts the f<strong>in</strong>d<strong>in</strong>gs of multiple regression<br />

only occasionally, and multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity problems show up very clearly. The<br />

most dramatic sign of a multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity problem is when your statistical program<br />

tells you that it cannot solve the equation. Some statistical programs calculate<br />

a condition <strong>in</strong>dex that diagnoses multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity. A condition <strong>in</strong>dex<br />

between 10 and 30 signals moderate to strong multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity, and an <strong>in</strong>dex<br />

over 30 signals a severe multicoll<strong>in</strong>earity problem (Gujarati 1995).<br />

But basic regression output shows the most common sign of a multicoll<strong>in</strong>-

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!