27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

48 Chapter 2<br />

between 70 and 80, and the difference between an IQ score of 90 and 100 is<br />

(assumed to be) the same as the difference between one of 130 and 140. On<br />

the other hand, 80 Fahrenheit is not twice as hot as 40, and a person who has<br />

an IQ of 150 is not 50% smarter than a person who has an IQ of 100.<br />

Ratio variables are <strong>in</strong>terval variables that have a true zero po<strong>in</strong>t—that is,<br />

a 0 that measures the absence of the phenomenon be<strong>in</strong>g measured. The Kelv<strong>in</strong><br />

scale of temperature has a true zero: It identifies the absence of molecular<br />

movement, or heat.<br />

The consequence of a true zero po<strong>in</strong>t is that measures have ratio properties.<br />

A person who is 40 years old is 10 years older than a person who is 30, and a<br />

person who is 20 is 10 years older than a person who is 10. The 10-year <strong>in</strong>tervals<br />

between the attributes (years are the attributes of age) are identical. That<br />

much is true of an <strong>in</strong>terval variable. In addition, however, a person who is 20<br />

is twice as old as a person who is 10; and a person who is 40 is twice as old<br />

as a person who is 20. These, then, are true ratios.<br />

While temperature (<strong>in</strong> Fahrenheit or Celsius) and IQ are nonratio <strong>in</strong>terval<br />

variables, most <strong>in</strong>terval-level variables <strong>in</strong> the social sciences are also ratio variables.<br />

In fact, it has become common practice <strong>in</strong> the social sciences to refer<br />

to ratio-level variables as <strong>in</strong>terval variables and vice versa. This is not technically<br />

pure, but the confusion of the terms ‘‘<strong>in</strong>terval’’ and ‘‘ratio’’ doesn’t cause<br />

much real damage.<br />

Some examples of ratio variables <strong>in</strong>clude: age, number of times a person<br />

has changed residence, <strong>in</strong>come <strong>in</strong> dollars or other currency, years married,<br />

years spent migrat<strong>in</strong>g, population size, distance <strong>in</strong> meters from a house to a<br />

well, number of hospital beds per million population, number of months s<strong>in</strong>ce<br />

last employment, number of kilograms of fish caught per week, number of<br />

hours per week spent <strong>in</strong> food preparation activities. Number of years of education<br />

is usually treated as a ratio variable, even though a year of grade school<br />

is hardly worth the same as a year of graduate school.<br />

In general, concepts (like alienation, political orientation, level of assimilation)<br />

are measured at the ord<strong>in</strong>al level. People get a high score for be<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘very<br />

assimilated,’’ a low score for be<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘unassimilated,’’ and a medium score for<br />

be<strong>in</strong>g ‘‘somewhat assimilated.’’ When a concept variable like <strong>in</strong>telligence is<br />

measured at the <strong>in</strong>terval level, it is likely to be the focus of a lot of controversy<br />

regard<strong>in</strong>g the validity of the measur<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong>strument.<br />

Concrete observables—th<strong>in</strong>gs you can actually see—are often measured at<br />

the <strong>in</strong>terval level. But not always. Observ<strong>in</strong>g whether a woman has a job outside<br />

her home is nom<strong>in</strong>al, qualitative measurement based on direct observation.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!