27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

314 Chapter 11<br />

terms of ‘‘those that are similar.’’ Some fieldworkers have used photographs<br />

of objects as stimuli for a pile sort.<br />

Borgatti (1999:133) po<strong>in</strong>ts out that physical stimuli, like images or objects,<br />

make people focus on form rather than function. In fact, when asked to sort<br />

draw<strong>in</strong>gs of fish, fishermen <strong>in</strong> North Carol<strong>in</strong>a sorted on shape—the long th<strong>in</strong><br />

ones, the ones with a big dorsal f<strong>in</strong>, the small roundish ones (Boster and Johnson<br />

1989). ‘‘In contrast,’’ says Borgatti (1999:133), ‘‘sort<strong>in</strong>g names of fish<br />

allows hidden attributes to affect the sort<strong>in</strong>g’’—th<strong>in</strong>gs like taste or how much<br />

of a struggle fish put up. ‘‘If you are after shared cultural beliefs,’’ says Borgatti,<br />

‘‘I recommend keep<strong>in</strong>g the stimulus as abstract as possible’’ (1992b:6).<br />

Pile Sorts and Taxonomic Trees<br />

Pile sort<strong>in</strong>g is an efficient method for generat<strong>in</strong>g taxonomic trees (Werner<br />

and Fenton 1973). Simply hand <strong>in</strong>formants the familiar pack of cards, each of<br />

which conta<strong>in</strong>s some term <strong>in</strong> a cultural doma<strong>in</strong>. Informants sort the cards <strong>in</strong>to<br />

piles, accord<strong>in</strong>g to whatever criterion makes sense to them. After the first sort<strong>in</strong>g,<br />

<strong>in</strong>formants are handed each pile and asked to go through the exercise<br />

aga<strong>in</strong>. They keep do<strong>in</strong>g this until they say that they cannot subdivide piles any<br />

further. At each sort<strong>in</strong>g level, <strong>in</strong>formants are asked if there is a word or phrase<br />

that describes each pile.<br />

Perchonock and Werner (1969) used this technique <strong>in</strong> their study of Navajo<br />

animal categories. After an <strong>in</strong>formant f<strong>in</strong>ished do<strong>in</strong>g a pile sort of animal<br />

terms, Perchonock and Werner built a branch<strong>in</strong>g tree diagram (such as that<br />

shown <strong>in</strong> figure 11.1) from the data. They would ask the <strong>in</strong>formant to make<br />

up sentences or phrases that expressed some relation between the nodes. They<br />

found that <strong>in</strong>formants <strong>in</strong>tuitively grasped the idea of tree representations for<br />

taxonomies. (For more about folk taxonomies, see chapter 18.)<br />

Pile Sorts and Networks<br />

I’ve used pile sorts to study the social structure of <strong>in</strong>stitutions such as prisons,<br />

ships at sea, and bureaucracies, and also to map the cognitively def<strong>in</strong>ed<br />

social organization of small communities. I simply hand people a deck of<br />

cards, each of which conta<strong>in</strong>s the name of one of the people <strong>in</strong> the group, and<br />

ask <strong>in</strong>formants to sort the cards <strong>in</strong>to piles, accord<strong>in</strong>g to their own criteria.<br />

The results tell me how people <strong>in</strong> the various components of an organization<br />

(managers, production workers, advertis<strong>in</strong>g people; or guards, counselors,<br />

prisoners; or seamen, deck officers, eng<strong>in</strong>e room personnel; or men and<br />

women <strong>in</strong> a small Greek village) th<strong>in</strong>k about the social structure of the group.<br />

Instead of ‘‘what goes with what,’’ I learn ‘‘who goes with whom.’’ Then I ask

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!