27.10.2014 Views

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

Russel-Research-Method-in-Anthropology

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

436 Chapter 15<br />

You may feel awkward about walk<strong>in</strong>g around with a clipboard (and perhaps<br />

a stopwatch) and writ<strong>in</strong>g down what people are do<strong>in</strong>g—or with beep<strong>in</strong>g people<br />

and ask<strong>in</strong>g them to <strong>in</strong>terrupt what they’re do<strong>in</strong>g to help you get some data.<br />

This is a reasonable concern, and direct observation is not for everyone. It’s<br />

not a detached method, like send<strong>in</strong>g out questionnaires and wait<strong>in</strong>g for data to<br />

be delivered to your doorstep.<br />

It is not a fun method, either. Hang<strong>in</strong>g out, participat<strong>in</strong>g <strong>in</strong> normal daily<br />

activities with people, and writ<strong>in</strong>g up field notes at night is more enjoyable<br />

than monitor<strong>in</strong>g and record<strong>in</strong>g what people are do<strong>in</strong>g.<br />

But many fieldworkers f<strong>in</strong>d that direct observation allows them to address<br />

issues that are not easily studied by any other method. Grace Marquis (1990)<br />

studied a shantytown <strong>in</strong> Lima, Peru. Children <strong>in</strong> households that kept chickens<br />

were at higher risk for gett<strong>in</strong>g diarrhea than were other children. The chickens<br />

left feces <strong>in</strong> the homes, and the feces conta<strong>in</strong>ed an organism that causes diarrhea.<br />

Cont<strong>in</strong>uous monitor<strong>in</strong>g showed that children touched the chicken dropp<strong>in</strong>gs<br />

and, <strong>in</strong>evitably, touched their mouths with their hands. It was hard,<br />

tedious work, but the payoff was serious.<br />

Direct observation is time consum<strong>in</strong>g, but random spot-check<strong>in</strong>g of behavior<br />

is a very cost effective and productive way to use some of your time <strong>in</strong> any<br />

field project. When you’re study<strong>in</strong>g a group that has clear boundaries (a village,<br />

a hospital, a school), you can get very f<strong>in</strong>e-gra<strong>in</strong>ed data about people’s<br />

behavior from a TA study, based on random spot checks. More importantly,<br />

as you can see from table 15.2, with proper sampl<strong>in</strong>g you can generalize to<br />

large populations (whole school districts, an entire aircraft manufactur<strong>in</strong>g<br />

plant, even cities) from spot checks of behavior, <strong>in</strong> ways that no other method<br />

allows.<br />

You may be concerned that a strictly observational approach to gather<strong>in</strong>g<br />

data about human behavior fails to capture the mean<strong>in</strong>g of data for the actors.<br />

This, too, is a legitimate concern. A classic example is Geertz’s (1973:6–7)<br />

observation that a w<strong>in</strong>k can be the result of gett<strong>in</strong>g a speck of dust <strong>in</strong> your eye<br />

or a conscious act of conspiracy. And that’s just a w<strong>in</strong>k. People can engage <strong>in</strong><br />

any of thousands of behaviors (skipp<strong>in</strong>g a class, wear<strong>in</strong>g a tie, hav<strong>in</strong>g their<br />

navel pierced . . .) for many, many different reasons. Know<strong>in</strong>g the mean<strong>in</strong>g of<br />

behavior to others is essential to understand<strong>in</strong>g it ourselves.<br />

On the other hand, one of our most important goals <strong>in</strong> science is to constantly<br />

challenge our own ideas about what th<strong>in</strong>gs mean. That’s how theories<br />

develop, are knocked down, and ga<strong>in</strong> <strong>in</strong> their power to expla<strong>in</strong> th<strong>in</strong>gs. Why<br />

shouldn’t we also challenge the theories—the explanations—that the people<br />

we study give us for their own behavior?<br />

Ask people who are com<strong>in</strong>g out of a church, for example, why they just<br />

spent 2 hours there. Some common responses <strong>in</strong>clude ‘‘to worship God,’’ ‘‘to

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!