21.01.2022 Views

Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences by Frederick J. Gravetter, Larry B. Wallnau ISBN 10: 1305504917 ISBN 13: 9781305504912

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

SECTION 11.1 | Introduction to Repeated-Measures Designs 337

The main advantage of a repeated-measures study is that it uses exactly the same

individuals in all treatment conditions. Thus, there is no risk that the participants in one

treatment are substantially different from the participants in another. With an independentmeasures

design, on the other hand, there is always a risk that the results are biased because

the individuals in one sample are systematically different (smarter, faster, more extroverted,

and so on) than the individuals in the other sample. At the end of this chapter, we present a

more detailed comparison of repeated-measures studies and independent-measures studies,

considering the advantages and disadvantages of both types of research.

■ The Matched-Subjects Design

Occasionally, researchers try to approximate the advantages of a repeated-measures design

by using a technique known as matched subjects. A matched-subjects design involves two

separate samples, but each individual in one sample is matched one-to-one with an individual

in the other sample. Typically, the individuals are matched on one or more variables

that are considered to be especially important for the study. For example, a researcher

studying verbal learning might want to be certain that the two samples are matched in

terms of IQ. In this case, a participant with an IQ of 120 in one sample would be matched

with another participant with an IQ of 120 in the other sample. Although the participants in

one sample are not identical to the participants in the other sample, the matched-subjects

design at least ensures that the two samples are equivalent (or matched) with respect to a

specific variable.

DEFINITION

In a matched-subjects study, each individual in one sample is matched with an

individual in the other sample. The matching is done so that the two individuals

are equivalent (or nearly equivalent) with respect to a specific variable that the

researcher would like to control.

Of course, it is possible to match participants on more than one variable. For example,

a researcher could match pairs of subjects on age, gender, race, and IQ. In this case, for

example, a 22-year-old white female with an IQ of 115 who was in one sample would be

matched with another 22-year-old white female with an IQ of 115 in the second sample.

The more variables that are used, however, the more difficult it is to find matching pairs.

The goal of the matching process is to simulate a repeated-measures design as closely as

possible. In a repeated-measures design, the matching is perfect because the same individual

is used in both conditions. In a matched-subjects design, however, the best you can

get is a degree of match that is limited to the variable(s) that are used for the matching

process.

In a repeated-measures design or a matched-subjects design comparing two treatment

conditions, the data consist of two sets of scores, which are grouped into sets of

two, corresponding to the two scores obtained for each individual or each matched pair

of subjects (Table 11.1). Because the scores in one set are directly related, one-to-one,

with the scores in the second set, the two research designs are statistically equivalent

and share the common name related-samples designs (or correlated-samples designs).

In this chapter, we focus our discussion on repeated-measures designs because they are

overwhelmingly the more common example of related-samples designs. However, you

should realize that the statistical techniques used for repeated-measures studies also can

be applied directly to data from a matched-subjects study. We should also note that a

matched-subjects study occasionally is called a matched samples design, but the subjects

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!