21.01.2022 Views

Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences by Frederick J. Gravetter, Larry B. Wallnau ISBN 10: 1305504917 ISBN 13: 9781305504912

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

SECTION 18.2 | An Example of the Binomial Test 609

population proportions is significantly greater than chance. That is, the data are not consistent

with the null hypothesis, so H 0

must be wrong. On the other hand, if the z-score is not

in the critical region, you fail to reject H 0

.

The following example demonstrates a complete binomial test.

EXAMPLE 18.1

In the Preview section, we described the visual cliff experiment designed to examine depth

perception in infants. To summarize briefly, an infant is placed on a wide board that appears

to have a deep drop on one side and a relatively shallow drop on the other. An infant who is

able to perceive depth should avoid the deep side and move toward the shallow side. Without

depth perception, the infant should show no preference between the two sides. Of the

27 infants in the experiment, 24 stayed exclusively on the shallow side and only 3 moved

onto the deep side. The purpose of the hypothesis test is to determine whether these data

demonstrate that infants have a significant preference for the shallow side.

This is a binomial hypothesis-testing situation. The two categories are

A = move onto the deep side

B = move onto the shallow side

STEP 1

The null hypothesis states that for the general population of infants, there is no preference

between the deep and the shallow sides; the direction of movement is determined by

chance. In symbols,

H 0

: p 5 psdeep sided 5 1 2 1 and q 5 1 22

H 1

: p ± 1 2

sThere is a preference.d

We will use α = .05.

STEP 2

STEP 3

With a sample of n = 27, pn = 13.5 and qn = 13.5. Both values are greater than 10, so

the distribution of z-scores is approximately normal. With α = .05, the critical region is

determined by boundaries of z = ± 1.96.

For this experiment, the data consist of X = 3 out of n = 27. Using Equation 18.1, these

data produce a z-score value of

z 5 X 2 pn

Ïnpq 5 3 2 13.5

Î27 1

1

221 1 22 5 210.5

2.60 524.04

To use Equation 18.2, you first compute the sample proportion, X/n = 3

27 = 0.111. The

z-score is then

z 5 Xyn 2 p

Ïpqyn

0.111 2 0.5

5

Î 1 21 1 5 20.389

22 y27 0.096 524.05

Within rounding error, the two equations produce the same result.

STEP 4

Because the data are in the critical region, our decision is to reject H 0

. These data do

provide sufficient evidence to conclude that there is a significant preference for the

shallow side. Gibson and Walk (1960) interpreted these data as convincing evidence that

depth perception is innate.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!