21.01.2022 Views

Statistics for the Behavioral Sciences by Frederick J. Gravetter, Larry B. Wallnau ISBN 10: 1305504917 ISBN 13: 9781305504912

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

Statistics is one of the most practical and essential courses that you will take, and a primary goal of this popular text is to make the task of learning statistics as simple as possible. Straightforward instruction, built-in learning aids, and real-world examples have made STATISTICS FOR THE BEHAVIORAL SCIENCES, 10th Edition the text selected most often by instructors for their students in the behavioral and social sciences. The authors provide a conceptual context that makes it easier to learn formulas and procedures, explaining why procedures were developed and when they should be used. This text will also instill the basic principles of objectivity and logic that are essential for science and valuable in everyday life, making it a useful reference long after you complete the course.

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

SECTION 15.3 | Using and Interpreting the Pearson Correlation 497

■ Interpreting Correlations

When you encounter correlations, there are four additional considerations that you should

bear in mind.

1. Correlation simply describes a relationship between two variables. It does not explain

why the two variables are related. Specifically, a correlation should not and cannot be

interpreted as proof of a cause-and-effect relationship between the two variables.

2. The value of a correlation can be affected greatly by the range of scores represented

in the data.

3. One or two extreme data points, often called outliers, can have a dramatic effect on

the value of a correlation.

4. When judging how “good” a relationship is, it is tempting to focus on the numerical

value of the correlation. For example, a correlation of +0.5 is halfway between

0 and 1.00 and therefore appears to represent a moderate degree of relationship.

However, a correlation should not be interpreted as a proportion. Although a correlation

of 1.00 does mean that there is a 100% perfectly predictable relationship

between X and Y, a correlation of .5 does not mean that you can make predictions

with 50% accuracy. To describe how accurately one variable predicts the other, you

must square the correlation. Thus, a correlation of r = .5 means that one variable

partially predicts the other, but the predictable portion is only r 2 = .5 2 = 0.25 (or

25%) of the total variability.

We now discuss each of these four points in detail.

■ Correlation and Causation

One of the most common errors in interpreting correlations is to assume that a correlation

necessarily implies a cause-and-effect relationship between the two variables. (Even Pearson

blundered by asserting causation from correlational data; Blum, 1978.) We constantly

are bombarded with reports of relationships: cigarette smoking is related to heart disease;

alcohol consumption is related to birth defects; carrot consumption is related to good eyesight.

Do these relationships mean that cigarettes cause heart disease or carrots cause good

eyesight? The answer is no. Although there may be a causal relationship, the simple existence

of a correlation does not prove it. Earlier, for example, we discussed a study showing

a relationship between high school grades and family income. However, this result does not

mean that having a higher family income causes students to get better grades. For example,

if mom gets an unexpected bonus at work, it is unlikely that her child’s grades will also

show a sudden increase. To establish a cause-and-effect relationship, it is necessary to

conduct a true experiment (see p. 13) in which one variable is manipulated by a researcher

and other variables are rigorously controlled. The fact that a correlation does not establish

causation is demonstrated in the following example.

EXAMPLE 15.4

Suppose we select a variety of different cities and towns throughout the United States and

measure the number of churches (X variable) and the number of serious crimes (Y variable)

for each. A scatter plot showing hypothetical data for this study is presented in Figure 15.5.

Notice that this scatter plot shows a strong, positive correlation between churches and

crime. You also should note that these are realistic data. It is reasonable that the small towns

would have less crime and fewer churches and that the large cities would have large values

for both variables. Does this relationship mean that churches cause crime? Does it mean

that crime causes churches? It should be clear that both answers are no. Although a strong

correlation exists between churches and crime, the real cause of the relationship is the size

of the population.

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!