09.02.2013 Views

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

Create successful ePaper yourself

Turn your PDF publications into a flip-book with our unique Google optimized e-Paper software.

Cantonese and Mandarin, we found that people use L1 is faster than L2 to name the picture, and<br />

Cantonese to Mandarin is slower than Mandarin to Cantonese. The results show that Mandarin is<br />

the dominant concept accessing ways for the bilinguals.<br />

1098.156Reflections on bilingual education in China from a psychology perspective, Hua-yu Tan,<br />

South China Universty, China<br />

In China, though bilingual education is still a fresh phenomenon, it has attracted wide-spread<br />

attention. The author <strong>of</strong> this paper makes a brief introduction <strong>of</strong> bilingual education and discusses<br />

some factors influencing the implementation <strong>of</strong> bilingual education in China from a psychology<br />

perspective, such as students' emotion, ages, mother tongue, learning strategies and the<br />

relationship between bilingual education and the development <strong>of</strong> students' intelligence.<br />

1098.157 Second language contrast acquisition and the impact <strong>of</strong> acculturation, Katherine<br />

Yoshida, Janet F. Werker, the University <strong>of</strong> British Columbia, Canada<br />

Languages have varying phonemic repertoires that contrast with one another to denote differences<br />

<strong>of</strong> meaning (e.g., the /r/-/l/ contrast is not valid in Japanese). Infants are born able to discriminate<br />

both native and non-native contrasts, yet by one-year only those in their native language continue<br />

to be perceived (Werker & Tees, 1984). The subsequent perception <strong>of</strong> non-native contrasts<br />

belonging to an ensuing language is less clearly understood. This work uses Cantonese-native ESL<br />

speakers to investigate the acquisition <strong>of</strong> non-native (English-specific) contrasts. Results <strong>of</strong> the<br />

English-Cantonese bilinguals will be discussed in the context <strong>of</strong> degree <strong>of</strong> acculturation to<br />

mainstream Canadian culture.<br />

1098.158 Adaptation to the facial expressions <strong>of</strong> emotion, Susumu Shibui, Kazuo Shigemasu,<br />

The University <strong>of</strong> Tokyo, Japan<br />

We investigated the categorization process <strong>of</strong> the facial expressions by means <strong>of</strong> the adaptation to<br />

the facial expressions <strong>of</strong> emotion. Subjects judged the expressions <strong>of</strong> 200 ms test faces following 5<br />

seconds viewing <strong>of</strong> an adaptation face. In the control condition, subjects judged only test faces.<br />

The effects <strong>of</strong> adaptation were represented by the delays <strong>of</strong> RTs between the control condition and<br />

the condition with adapt faces. The result suggested that the effect <strong>of</strong> adaptation was explained by<br />

the categorical distances between the test faces and adapt faces. This is in line with the categorical<br />

model <strong>of</strong> the facial expression.<br />

1098.159 Dismissing-avoidant pattern <strong>of</strong> attachment related to facial mimicry, emotion regulation,<br />

and empathy, Marianne Sonnby-Borgstrom, Peter Jonsson, Department <strong>of</strong> <strong>Psychology</strong>, Lund,<br />

Sweden<br />

Dismissing-avoidant individuals are assumed to repress anxiety, a disposition hypothesized to<br />

interfere with facial mimicry and empathy. Mimicry (electromyography) was compared in<br />

dismissing-avoidant and non-dismissing individuals. Happy or angry faces were exposed at<br />

different levels <strong>of</strong> information processing. At the automatic level (56ms) dismissing-avoidant<br />

participants showed “normal” corrugator responses (negative emotions) upon exposure to angry<br />

faces. At the controlled level (2350 ms) they showed no corrugator response and "smiling<br />

reactions” to angry faces, whereas the non-dismissing participants reacted with mimicking and<br />

scored also significantly higher on emotional empathy. "Smiling" at the controlled level may be<br />

245

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!