09.02.2013 Views

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

28th International Congress of Psychology August 8 ... - U-netSURF

SHOW MORE
SHOW LESS

You also want an ePaper? Increase the reach of your titles

YUMPU automatically turns print PDFs into web optimized ePapers that Google loves.

This paper examines beliefs about the causes <strong>of</strong> Bulimia Nervosa held by the general public,<br />

university students and pr<strong>of</strong>essional groups involved in the diagnosis and management <strong>of</strong> this<br />

condition. Pr<strong>of</strong>essional groups included psychiatrists, general practitioners, psychologists, social<br />

workers, mental health nurses and dieticians. Participants rated 44 potential causal explanations <strong>of</strong><br />

Bulimia. Principal components analysis was used to reduce the causal explanations to a smaller set<br />

<strong>of</strong> underlying dimensions. The pr<strong>of</strong>essional and non-pr<strong>of</strong>essional groups were compared on their<br />

mean endorsement <strong>of</strong> these dimensions. Implications <strong>of</strong> differences in causal beliefs between<br />

pr<strong>of</strong>essional and non-pr<strong>of</strong>essional groups for treatment acceptance and compliance are discussed.<br />

3118.71 Effects <strong>of</strong> affective states on verbal communication, Ai Kato, Minoru Karasawa,<br />

Junko Ohshita, Kobe University, Japan<br />

The present study examined how affect influences the use <strong>of</strong> language. On the basis <strong>of</strong> the Affect<br />

Infusion Model (Forgas, 1995), we predicted that negative affect, as compared to positive affect,<br />

would increase politeness when the speaker makes a request. Japanese undergraduates received an<br />

affect manipulation by means <strong>of</strong> success/failure feedback on an achievement task and by a<br />

background visual stimulus. They then responded verbally to a scenario <strong>of</strong> request making. The<br />

results partly supported the hypothesis and revealed a gender difference. The influence <strong>of</strong> the<br />

seniority norm in Japanese culture is also discussed.<br />

3118.72 Young children’s beliefs about the stability <strong>of</strong> traits, Wen Gao, Li-zhu Yang, liaoning<br />

Normal University, China<br />

Prior research has demonstrated individual differences in children's beliefs about the stability <strong>of</strong><br />

traits, but this focus on developmental differences about it. In a series <strong>of</strong> experiments, we<br />

individually investigated 280 Chinese young children, found the youngest children (3-4 years old)<br />

could not understand traits, the younger children (5-6 years old)were optimistic in their beliefs<br />

about traits and the oldest children (7-9 years old) began to learn some traits were malleable. The<br />

sources <strong>of</strong> young children's optimism and implications <strong>of</strong> this optimism for age differences are<br />

discussed.<br />

3118.73 The interpretive theory <strong>of</strong> mind and peer rejection in adolescence, Hongmei Zhao,<br />

Yanjie Su, Peking University, Beijing, China<br />

This study amied to investigate whether and how the ways that adolescents used the "interpretive<br />

theory <strong>of</strong> mind" were linked to their sociometric status. 376 adolescents aged 12 to 15 performed<br />

four tasks about interpretation <strong>of</strong> peer rejection event. Results showed that peer rejected<br />

adolescents presented a similar way <strong>of</strong> interpretation to other peers' rejected events, however they<br />

interprete their own rejected events in a completely different way compared with popular,<br />

negelected and average adolescents. It was suggested that the ways the rejected adolescents used<br />

to interpret other people's behaviors may related to their levels <strong>of</strong> peer acceptance.<br />

3118.74 How do people make social judgements based on summarized information? Bert<br />

Timmermans, Frank Van Overwalle, Vrije Universiteit Brussel, Belgium<br />

How do people process summarized social information as opposed to on-line information <strong>of</strong><br />

separate exemplars or instances? We suggest that, at least under some conditions, when making<br />

trait- or category judgments based on summary information, people first "transform" this<br />

767

Hooray! Your file is uploaded and ready to be published.

Saved successfully!

Ooh no, something went wrong!